Journal Information
Vol. 93. Issue 3.
Pages 253-259 (May - June 2017)
Visits
3676
Vol. 93. Issue 3.
Pages 253-259 (May - June 2017)
Original article
Open Access
A method for the assessment of facial hedonic reactions in newborns
Método para a avaliação de reações faciais hedônicas em recém-nascidos
Visits
3676
Caroline Ayresa,b,
Corresponding author
nutricarolayres@hotmail.com

Corresponding author.
, Charles F. Ferreiraa,b,c, Juliana R. Bernardia,b,d, Thiago B. Marcelinoa,b, Vânia N. Hirakatae, Clécio H. da Silvaa,b, Marcelo Z. Goldania,b
a Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Faculdade de Medicina, Núcleo de Estudos em Saúde da Criança e do Adolescente (NESCA), Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
b Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Faculdade de Medicina, Pós-Graduação em Saúde da Criança e do Adolescente, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
c Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Faculdade de Medicina, Pós-Graduação em Ciências da Saúde: Ginecologia e Obstetrícia (PPGGO), Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
d Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Faculdade de Medicina, Departamento de Nutrição, Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
e Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Faculdade de Medicina, Grupo de Pesquisa e Pós-Graduac¸ão (GPPG), Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil
This item has received

Under a Creative Commons license
Article information
Abstract
Full Text
Bibliography
Download PDF
Statistics
Figures (4)
Show moreShow less
Abstract
Objective

This study describes a quantitative and qualitative methodology to assess hedonic responses to sweet stimulus in healthy newborns.

Methods

A descriptive, cross-sectional, observational study, with healthy newborns (up to 24h of life), between 37 and 42 gestational weeks, vaginally born and breastfed previously to all tests. The evaluation of the newborns reactions was performed by hedonic facial expression analysis, characterized by facial expressions with rhythmic serial tongue protrusion after neutral or sweet solution intake. Initially, 1mL of water solution was provided to the newborn, followed by a 1-minute recording. Afterwards, the same amount of 25% sucrose solution was provided, performing a second recording. The concordance between researchers was analyzed by the Bland–Altman statistical method.

Results

A total of 100 newborns (n=49 males, n=51 females; mean lifetime=15h 12min±6h 29min) were recorded for neutral and sucrose solution intake, totaling 197 videos (n=3 missing in the water treatment). These videos were double-blind analyzed and the test revealed a 90% concordance between the two trained researchers, in relation to both solutions. The intraclass correlation coefficient was 0.99 for both solutions, with a significant increase in frequency of hedonic expressions evoked by sucrose solution intake.

Conclusions

These results confirm that the proposed method has an efficient power to detect significant differences between neutral and sucrose stimuli. In conclusion, this evaluation method of hedonic facial reactions in newborns reflects the response to a specific taste.

Keywords:
Newborn
Facial expression
Sucrose
Resumo
Objetivo

Descrever quantitativamente e qualitativamente uma metodologia para avaliar as respostas faciais hedônicas, em recém-nascidos saudáveis, ao estímulo doce.

Métodos

Trata-se de um estudo descritivo, transversal e observacional, com recém-nascidos saudáveis (com até 24 horas de vida), entre 37-42 semanas gestacionais, nascidos por parto vaginal e alimentados previamente aos testes. A avaliação das reações hedônicas dos recém-nascidos foi considerada pelas expressões faciais com séries rítmicas de protrusões de língua após a ingestão de solução neutra ou doce. Inicialmente, 1mL de solução neutra (água) foi fornecida para o recém-nascido, seguido de uma filmagem de 1 minuto. Sequencialmente, a mesma quantidade de solução de sacarose 25% foi fornecida, realizando-se uma segunda gravação. A concordância entre os pesquisadores foi analisada pelo método estatístico de Bland-Altman.

Resultados

Um total de 100 recém-nascidos (n=49 do sexo masculino, n=51 do sexo feminino, tempo de vida média=15h 12min±6h 29min) foram registrados para a ingestão de solução neutra e de sacarose, totalizando 197 vídeos (n=3 perdas para o tratamento água). Estes vídeos foram analisados em duplo-cego e o teste revelou uma concordância de 90%, para ambas as soluções, entre os pesquisadores treinados. O coeficiente de correlação intraclasse foi de 0,99 para as duas substâncias, com um aumento significativo nas frequências das expressões faciais hedônicas evocadas pela ingestão de sacarose.

Conclusões

Estes resultados confirmam que o método proposto possui poder estatístico eficiente para detectar diferenças entre estímulos neutros e sacarose. Em conclusão, este método de avaliação de reações faciais hedônicas em recém-nascidos reflete a resposta para um gosto específico.

Palavras-chave:
Recém-nascido
Expressão facial
Sacarose
Full Text
Introduction

Evidence suggests that affective reactions reflect the quality of pleasant or unpleasant events, since newborns have essentially two patterns of facial expressions to taste: positive emotional reaction – hedonic, or negative emotional reaction – disgust. The sweet taste of sugar usually draws positive affective patterns, such as a “nozzle” lip-shape and a rhythmic series of tongue protrusion movements. These movements are accompanied by relaxation of facial muscles.1 Some studies have reported that the experience of a taste during intrauterine life could improve the acceptance of foods with the same taste in childhood.2–5 The automatic responses to stimulus can be innate, such as in sweet taste,6 and may change during the course of life, depending on the type of in utero exposure, among other factors before and after birth.2,7 The child's facial expression resulting from a sweet taste is an example of a positive affective reaction behavior.1,8 In addition, hedonic expressions reflect the activity of specific mesolimbic systems,9 which are also activated after visualization of tasty foods.10 As variations in this circuit's responsiveness may predict the risk for weight gain,11 behavioral alterations could also indicate a risk for overeating and/or overweight in the future.

There are different methods in the literature to evaluate the hedonic responses to gustatory stimuli in children. In the early 1970s, Steiner was the pioneering author in studies about affective reactivity with the publication of illustrations of the facial reactions in newborns evoked by the sweet, salty, sour, and bitter tastes.8,12 In 1976, Crook and Lipsitt conducted four experiments about the effects of brief intraoral fluid stimulation upon the non-nutritive sucking rhythm of newborns.13 Beauchamp and Moran, in 1982, studied the sucrose preference at birth determined by allowing ad libitum consumption of sucrose and water solutions during brief presentations, according to the dietary record.6 Ganchrow et al., in 1983, evaluated newborns’ facial expressions recorded by video after oral stimulation with distilled water and sucrose, urea, and quinine hydrochloride.14 In 1988, Rosenstein and Oster evaluated videotaped facial expressions based on the Facial Action Coding System adapted for infants (Baby FACS) evoked by sucrose, sodium chloride, citric acid, and quinine hydrochloride.15 In 1993, Porges and Lipsitt analyzed the gustatory-vagal hypothesis regarding neonatal responsivity to gustatory stimulation.16 In this sense, taste reactivity research demonstrated that sucrose evoked hedonic patterns (e.g., lip smacking and rhythmic series of tongue protrusion, accompanied by relaxation of the facial muscles and an occasional smile).1,17,18 Recently, in 2015, Zacche Sa et al. analyzed facial responses to basic tastes among newborns of women with and without gestational diabetes mellitus.19 Some of these studies recorded facial expressions through filming techniques, allowing a precise evaluation of the frequency of hedonic and aversive reactions, as well as its duration. Of all the aforementioned research, some of these investigations1,8,12,14–16,19 can be compared to the present manuscript. However, this study methodologically resembles only that of Ganchrow et al. in 1983, with a larger sample size.14 It is noteworthy that in all these comparative studies, there was no report of statistical analysis between the two researchers. In this sense, this research aimed to describe a quantitative and qualitative methodology able to assess the hedonic responses to sweet stimulus in healthy newborns analyzed by double-blinded, trained researchers, using the Bland–Altman statistical method.

Methods

This was a cross-sectional, observational study, presenting a descriptive manuscript focusing on an adapted method from studies by Berridge, Steiner, and Ganchrow1,14,17 to assess the facial reactions to sweet taste in newborns. Data was collected from November 2010 to May 2012, in a hospital (Grupo Hospitalar Conceição – GHC) located in Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brazil, which assists the Unified Health System.

The convenience sample included only healthy newborns, up to 24h of life, at 37–42 gestational weeks, vaginally delivered and breastfed before all treatments. Children of mothers who did not sign the informed consent, cases of maternal diseases (e.g., diabetes, hypertension, HIV), and newborns who were not exclusively breastfed were excluded from sample. Maternal and newborn data were collected from medical records and from the prenatal child health handbook.

All procedures performed in this study involved human participants and were in accordance to the ethical standards of GHC research Ethics Committee and to the Helsinki Declaration of 1964 and its later amendments.

Hedonic reactions or positive emotional facial reactions

The newborns’ positive affective facial reaction assessments were performed by hedonic facial expression analysis, characterized by serial and rhythmic tongue protrusion through fresh or neutral solution intake.1,8,12

Initially, 1mL of neutral solution (deionized water) was provided to the newborn, followed by a 1min recording. After this first recording, a second filming was performed after 1mL of water+25% sucrose solution was administrated. It is emphasized that the neutral solution was first provided to all newborns, and that sucrose recordings were performed only after water testing. All solutions were administrated orally, pressing the newborns’ masseter muscle lightly, opening their mouths and allowing administration. The facial expressions were recorded continuously through 1min, with a 20-cm distance between the camera and the newborn, under natural light inside the hospital room.

A Cyber-Shot DSC-S500 camera (Sony®, Tokyo, Japan), with 6.0 mega pixels and 30 frames per second, was used for recording all tests. Newborns were placed in a comfortable position, without crying, near their mothers. Before water recordings, they were previously breastfed. After the tests, mothers were oriented toward the practice of newborn oral care, as well as encouraged to practice exclusive breastfeeding through explanations of its importance and benefits for both the mother and the baby.

Solutions

Deionized water was used as a neutral solution and the sucrose solution was composed of 1mL of deionized water containing 0.25g of sucrose – modified from Steiner et al. in 2001 and Ayres et al. in 2012.1,20

Solutions were produced by a compounding pharmacy. The deionized water was subjected to three purification processes: sediment removal, bacteria elimination, and total elimination of micronutrients. All solutions were individually packaged in a bottle dropper; labeled with the name of the solution, quantity, and expiry date; and finally sealed and individually wrapped in a plastic bag. These solutions were produced daily and delivered on the same morning, since data collection was performed in the afternoons. In this interval, all solutions were kept under refrigeration at 2–8°C, as their validity was restricted to 24h after preparation. If not used during this period, solutions were discarded. All recipients were opened in the presence of the mother, immediately prior to the testing moment. At the testing time, they were kept at room temperature (between 15°C and 25°C).

Video analysis

All 1-minute videos were firstly analyzed at a normal speed by two trained researchers, to verify whether there were hedonic facial expressions during the one-minute recording. Later, all records were evaluated frame-by-frame using Windows Media Player software (Microsoft®, WA, USA), slowing the speed to better detect hedonic facial expressions. Every second was composed of 30 frames, totalling 1800 frames per 60seconds. The frequency of facial reactions in 2-s intervals was recorded for 1min of continuous shooting. Facial expressions representing hedonic reactions or positive affective reactions were considered. For example, for each hedonic facial expression or tongue protrusion, a frequency was registered. For the analysis of these results, the sums of all 60-s records were calculated.1,17

Hedonic reactions were characterized by facial expressions such as lip suctions and rhythmic tongue-protrusion movements.8 Videos were analyzed by two double-blinded, trained researchers, both trained by another researcher who had already used this technique.

Sample size estimation

Martin Bland and Doug Altman, authors of the statistical test to assess agreement between evaluators (concordance analysis using the Bland–Altman method), state that for an adequate accuracy it is important to evaluate 100 individuals, corresponding to a 95% confidence interval and a 0.34 standard error of deviation.21

Statistical methods

Symmetric data were expressed as mean and standard deviation (SD) and asymmetric data were expressed as median and interquartile ranges. To evaluate the agreement between the two raters, a concordance between Bland–Altman method was applied. This methodology evaluates the correlation between two variables (X and Y) starting from a graphical view based on a scatter plot of the difference of the two variables (XY) and their average (X+Y/2). In this chart, it is possible to visualize the bias (how the differences derivate from zero), the error (dispersion of the points around the mean), the outliers, and possible trends.21 In order to complement these analyses, an intraclass coefficient (ICC) was also estimated. To compare the frequency of hedonic facial expressions, values evoked by water and sucrose solutions were analyzed by the Wilcoxon test. All data were evaluated using Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS, Inc. version 18.0, IL, USA), version 18.0. The level of significance was set at 5% for all analyses.

Results

The newborns’ average life was 15h 12min±6h 29min hours and minutes. Among the 100 newborns evaluated, 49% were male and 51% were female. Birth weight average was 3274.9±439.85g, with 39±1 weeks of gestational age. The mean maternal age was 23±6 years.

A total of 100 newborns were recorded for neutral (water) and sucrose solution intake, generating 197 videos (n=3 missing data for the water treatment). Fig. 1 displays four newborns sequences for neutral (Fig. 1A) and sucrose (Fig. 1B) solutions.

Figure 1.

Newborn facial expressions to neutral (water, A) and (B) sucrose solutions. (A) Characteristic newborn facial expression to neutral solution (water). (B) Characteristic newborn facial expression to sucrose solution, exhibiting tongue protrusions (designated as hedonic reaction when performed in rhythmic series). Previously to the administration of both solutions (water and sucrose), all newborns showed no spontaneous facial hedonic responses. The intake of both solutions induced hedonic reaction expressions, but the sucrose solution evoked higher frequencies of hedonic reaction expressions in relation to intake of the neutral solution.

Photos representing newborn's baseline (pre-treatment), during solution administration, first (if it occurred) and last protrusion after solution administration (in minutes and seconds).

(0.1MB).

The Bland–Altman test indicated agreement between researchers, both in relation to the water solution (Fig. 2) and the sucrose solution (Fig. 3). Through 95% agreement limits, it was observed that there was an agreement in more than 90% of the data.

Figure 2.

Bland–Altman dispersion chart representing inter-rater agreement in relation to neutral solution (water). AUL, agreement upper limit; ALL, agreement lower limit.

(0.09MB).
Figure 3.

Bland–Altman dispersion chart representing inter-rater agreement in relation to sucrose solution. AUL, agreement upper limit; ALL, agreement lower limit.

(0.09MB).

Previously to the administration of both solutions (water and sucrose), all newborns showed no spontaneous facial hedonic responses. Total hedonic responses evoked by neutral solution (water) was scored 0 for both researchers (researcher 1: median=0.0, 25th percentile=0.0, 75th percentile=48.5; researcher 2: median=0.0, 25th percentile=0.0, 75th percentile=48.5). On the other hand, hedonic responses evoked by sucrose solution were scored 17 and 16.0 for both researchers, respectively (researcher 1: median=17.0, 25th percentile=0.0, 75th percentile=98.0; researcher 2: median=16.0, 25th percentile=0.0, 75th percentile=96.8). The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) for all analyses was 0.99 for both (p0.001).

Comparing hedonic facial frequency values between neutral and sucrose solutions, an increase in hedonic facial expression evoked by the sucrose solution (p=0.004) was observed. This result confirms that this methodology displays power to detect differences between sucrose and neutral stimuli (Fig. 4).

Figure 4.

Frequencies of hedonic expressions in response to neutral (water) or sucrose solutions. Hedonic facial frequencies were increased in response to sucrose solution, compared to the neutral solution (p=0.004). The box represents the interquartile (IQ) range. The whiskers are lines that extend from the upper and lower edge of the box. A line across the box indicates the median. Outliers are cases with values between 1.5 and 3 times the IQ range (¿). Extremes are cases with values more than 3 times the IQ range (*).

(0.05MB).
Discussion

To the best of authors’ knowledge, this is the first Brazilian protocol showing an assessment method description for newborns’ (up to 24h of life) affective facial reactions through sweet taste stimulus involving recorded facial expression analysis. This study represents national comparative research (by two researchers blinded to the solutions analyzed – water and sucrose) using a detailed methodology for the qualification of hedonic facial reactions. This study provides a national scientific protocol with reliable reproducibility, considering the degree of statistical agreement between the two researchers, confirmed by the Bland–Altman method. It was observed that the sweet solution evoked hedonic reactions in infants and that facial expression is an important measure to evaluate possible hedonic responses. The statistical inter-rater agreement confirms that this method is an effective protocol for measuring the hedonic impact of sweet taste in newborns.

Some researchers have recorded facial expressions from photographs, thus analyzing affective reactions of infants and animals.8,12 With available technology, researchers were able to videotape facial expressions through recordings, which allowed an accurate assessment of facial expression variables (e.g., frequency, duration), such as hedonic and aversive reactions. Furthermore, this method allows for reducing the video speed and evaluating parameters frame-by-frame. It is also possible to identify the hedonic responses to taste stimuli in rodents and primates. Additionally, Mennella et al. described that fetuses exhibit hedonic responses through facial expressions for tastes, even before birth.22 Some authors have already studied children's response to sucrose stimulus, suggesting that an early experience to sweetened water intake could maintain a preference for sucrose solutions later in life.6 These expressions induced by tastes produce feelings, as this parameter refers only to taste quality, not measuring its intensity. Facial expressions are affective reactions for a specific taste hedonic impact/response, reflecting the pleasure feeling of a particular taste. Additionally, it is not possible to assess the substance offered only by its sensory quality. A salty taste, for example, could extract a positive reaction in the same way that a sweet taste does. On the other hand, a sour, bitter, or very salty taste could extract similar negative reactions. However, a trained researcher could assess whether a child liked a taste based on facial expression, considering this as a positive (hedonic) or a negative (aversive) reaction.17

In 2009, Schwartz et al. assessed the acceptance of tastes and their developmental changes over the first year, compared to acceptance for all tastes and reactivity to the overall flavor.23 The acceptance of tastes (sweet, salty, bitter, sour, and umami) was evaluated in three groups (45 subjects) with ages of 3, 6, and 12 months. In all ages, the acceptance during the first year and the most preferred taste was confirmed by the sweet and salty taste. Facial reactions to umami were neutral. Sour and bitter tastes were the least accepted. By improving the knowledge of the acceptance of a certain taste, a better understanding of feeding behavior in childhood is provided, which could reflect the eating behavior in adulthood.

Focusing on the study of facial expressions, studies have shown that some taste components induces similar facial reactions among all primates, such as “gapes” for bitter flavors, or serial and rhythmic tongue protrusions for sweets.1,17,18 Another study aimed to compare the facial responses to tastes among newborns of women with and without gestational diabetes mellitus. The tastes were evaluated by glucose, sodium chloride, citric acid, and quinine hydrochloride administrations; 0.2mL of solution over the dorsal surface of the tongue. In this study, the newborns had their faces and facial responses recorded were coded according to the Baby FACS, in which newborns of mothers with gestational diabetes mellitus seem to prefer the taste of salt.19

Although the preference for sweet substances exists in newborns, standard experiences with sweet foods and drinks during development could be modulated. Besides, few studies have investigated the direct relationship between eating sweets and their real effect on the appetite for sweet tastes in newborns.4–7 Studies that investigated the neural basis for sensory pleasure related to food designated some brain sub-regions as “hedonic hotspots,” which are encephalic areas capable of amplifying causally positive affective reactions to sweet tastes in response to certain neurobiological and neurochemical stimuli (e.g., orexin, opioids, palatable foods, and sweet tastes), generating “liking” or “pleasurable” feelings.9,24–26 These hedonic mechanisms are at least partially distinct from mesocorticolimbic circuitry, a biological pathway that generates motivation for eating and the “wanting” feeling.24,25 The current evidence suggests that “hedonic hotspots” exist in limbic brain structures, being capable of increasing the hedonic impact of natural sensory rewards, like palatable and sweet tastes. In this sense, these access points are found in nucleus accumbens (particularly its medial shell area),24,25 ventral pallidum,26 and parabrachial nucleus.24 Besides, some regions could amplify the sweet hedonic impact, expressing increased “liking/pleasure” reactions for the taste of sucrose.26

As reported by Kringelbach and Berridge, eating behavior is one of the basic life pleasures, which could be modulated by what is eaten or enjoyed in different ways (e.g., liking and disliking).27 These personal differences are linked to the individual learning of liking and disliking a specific taste, in addition to an innate preference predisposed to the basic tastes. Some extrinsic, intrinsic, and individual factors may contribute to taste preference, as they are associated to subjective experiences or modulated by hunger/satiety states.28 This subjective taste perception is produced by different signals to the central nervous system areas.29,30 Taken together, these data support important information focusing on the representation of taste preference in the central nervous system.

Affective reactivity to taste as a tool for measuring hedonic functions by test could be applied as a useful and objective tool for measurement of liking/pleasure hedonic reactions in response to palatable food, based on the quantification of different taste-induced orofacial affective reactions.1

This research presents some limitations, such as not having added in the sample newborns who were more than 24h away from mothers in the hospital rooming. Additionally, another limitation concerns the substances studied. As mentioned before, other substances than sucrose (e.g., salty, sour, or bitter tastes) could be used in this assessment. However, this research's focus was on facial hedonic reactions evoked by sucrose, justifying its specific use. Thus, despite these limitations, the authors can highlight some important features of this research protocol: this study was national comparative research (by two researchers blinded to the substances analyzed–water and sucrose) using a detailed methodology for hedonic facial reaction quantification, using an adequate newborn sample size. Only exclusively breastfed newborns were included in this sample; all newborns were breastfed previously to the treatments. This research provides a national scientific protocol with reliable reproducibility of findings, considering the degree of statistical agreement between the two researchers, confirmed by the Bland–Altman method.

Considering the results, the hedonic facial reactions in newborns reflect the hedonic impact for this specific taste, allowing this methodology to be applied in early life, but in already responsive stages. The authors speculate that hedonic responses through facial expressions are related to the feeling of pleasure due to taste, allowing this direct relation to be interpreted as a greater food consumption of this taste type later in life. More evidence concerning this neural pleasure/reward circuitry relationship is necessary, since it is capable of modifying food intake by “liking/pleasure” responses to foods or tastes, without considering the nutritional balance or nutritional needs. As reward pathways are involved, other pleasurable stimuli may replace the specific consumption of palatable foods that acts through these routes.

Funding

Supported by the Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq), Brazil.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References
[1]
J.E. Steiner, D. Glaser, M.E. Hawilo, K.C. Berridge.
Comparative expression of hedonic impact: affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 25 (2001), pp. 53-74
[2]
J.A. Mennella.
Flavour programming during breast-feeding.
Adv Exp Med Biol, 639 (2009), pp. 113-120
[3]
J.A. Mennella, M.Y. Pepino, D.R. Reed.
Genetic and environmental determinants of bitter perception and sweet preferences.
Pediatrics, 115 (2005), pp. e216-e222
[4]
J.A. Mennella, C.E. Griffin, G.K. Beauchamp.
Flavor programming during infancy.
Pediatrics, 113 (2004), pp. 840-845
[5]
J.A. Mennella, C.P. Jagnow, G.K. Beauchamp.
Prenatal and postnatal flavor learning by human infants.
Pediatrics, 107 (2001), pp. E88
[6]
G.K. Beauchamp, M. Moran.
Dietary experience and sweet taste preference in human infants.
Appetite, 3 (1982), pp. 139-152
[7]
G.K. Beauchamp, J.A. Mennella.
Early flavor learning and its impact on later feeding behavior.
J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr, 48 (2009), pp. S25-S30
[8]
J.E. Steiner.
The gustofacial response: observation on normal and anencephalic newborn infants.
Symp Oral Sens Percept, (1973), pp. 254-278
[9]
S. Peciña, K.S. Smith, K.C. Berridge.
Hedonic hot spots in the brain.
Neuroscientist, 12 (2006), pp. 500-511
[10]
N.D. Volkow, J.S. Fowler.
Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex.
Cereb Cortex, 10 (2000), pp. 318-325
[11]
E. Stice, S. Yokum, C. Bohon, N. Marti, A. Smolen.
Reward circuitry responsivity to food predicts future increases in body mass: moderating effects of DRD2 and DRD4.
Neuroimage, 50 (2010), pp. 1618-1625
[12]
J.E. Steiner.
Discussion paper: innate, discriminative human facial expressions to taste and smell stimulation.
Ann N Y Acad Sci, 237 (1974), pp. 229-233
[13]
C.K. Crook, L.P. Lipsitt.
Neonatal nutritive sucking: effects of taste stimulation upon sucking rhythm and heart rate.
Child Dev, 47 (1976), pp. 518-522
[14]
J.R. Ganchrow, J.E. Steiner, M. Daher.
Neonatal facial expressions in response to different qualities and intensities of gustatory stimulation in response to different qualities and intensities of gustatory stimuli.
Infant Behav Dev, 6 (1983), pp. 473-484
[15]
D. Rosenstein, H. Oster.
Differential facial responses to four basic tastes in newborns.
Child Dev, 59 (1988), pp. 1555-1568
[16]
S.W. Porges, L.P. Lipsitt.
Neonatal responsivity to gustatory stimulation: the gustatory-vagal hypothesis.
Infant Behav Dev, 16 (1993), pp. 487-494
[17]
K.C. Berridge.
Measuring hedonic impact in animals and infants: microstructure of affective taste reactivity patterns.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 24 (2000), pp. 173-198
[18]
K.S. Smith, K.C. Berridge.
Opioid limbic circuit for reward: interaction between hedonic hotspots of nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum.
J Neurosci, 27 (2007), pp. 1594-1605
[19]
A. Zacche Sa, J.R. Silva, J.G. Alves.
Facial responses to basic tastes in the newborns of women with gestational diabetes mellitus.
J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med, 28 (2015), pp. 1687-1690
[20]
C. Ayres, M. Agranonik, A.K. Portella, F. Filion, C.C. Johnston, P.P. Silveira.
Intrauterine growth restriction and the fetal programming of the hedonic response to sweet taste in newborn infants.
Int J Pediatr, 2012 (2012), pp. 6573-6579
[21]
J.M. Bland, D.G. Altman.
Agreed statistics: measurement method comparison.
Anesthesiology, 116 (2012), pp. 182-185
[22]
J.A. Mennella, A. Johnson, G.K. Beauchamp.
Garlic ingestion by pregnant women alters the odor of amniotic fluid.
Chem Senses, 20 (1995), pp. 207-209
[23]
C. Schwartz, S. Issanchou, S. Nicklaus.
Developmental changes in the acceptance of the five basic tastes in the first year of life.
Br J Nutr, 102 (2009), pp. 1375-1385
[24]
D.C. Castro, K.C. Berridge.
Advances in the neurobiological bases for food ‘liking’ versus ‘wanting’.
Physiol Behav, 136 (2014), pp. 22-30
[25]
D.C. Castro, K.C. Berridge.
Opioid hedonic hotspot in nucleus accumbens shell: mu, delta, and kappa maps for enhancement of sweetness “liking” and “wanting”.
J Neurosci, 34 (2014), pp. 4239-4250
[26]
C.Y. Ho, K.C. Berridge.
An orexin hotspot in ventral pallidum amplifies hedonic ‘liking’ for sweetness.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 38 (2013), pp. 1655-1664
[27]
B.E. Leonard.
Pleasures of the Brain Edited by Morten L. Kringelbach, Kent C. Berridge, Oxford University Press, Oxford. pp 343, ISBN: 978-0-533102-8.
Hum Psychopharmacol Clin Exp, 25 (2010), pp. 429
[28]
D.M. Small, R.J. Zatorre, A. Dagher, A.C. Evans, M. Jones-Gotman.
Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion.
Brain, 124 (2001), pp. 1720-1733
[29]
M.L. Kringelbach.
The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience.
Nat Rev Neurosci, 6 (2005), pp. 691-702
[30]
D.M. Small.
Flavor is in the brain.
Physiol Behav, 107 (2012), pp. 540-552

Please cite this article as: Ayres C, Ferreira CF, Bernardi JR, Marcelino TB, Hirakata VN, Silva CH, et al. A method for the assessment of facial hedonic reactions in newborns. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2017;93:253–9.

Copyright © 2016. Sociedade Brasileira de Pediatria
Download PDF
Idiomas
Jornal de Pediatria (English Edition)
Article options
Tools
en pt
Taxa de publicaçao Publication fee
Os artigos submetidos a partir de 1º de setembro de 2018, que forem aceitos para publicação no Jornal de Pediatria, estarão sujeitos a uma taxa para que tenham sua publicação garantida. O artigo aceito somente será publicado após a comprovação do pagamento da taxa de publicação. Ao submeterem o manuscrito a este jornal, os autores concordam com esses termos. A submissão dos manuscritos continua gratuita. Para mais informações, contate jped2@sbp.com.br. Articles submitted as of September 1, 2018, which are accepted for publication in the Jornal de Pediatria, will be subject to a fee to have their publication guaranteed. The accepted article will only be published after proof of the publication fee payment. By submitting the manuscript to this journal, the authors agree to these terms. Manuscript submission remains free of charge. For more information, contact jped2@sbp.com.br.
Cookies policy Política de cookies
To improve our services and products, we use "cookies" (own or third parties authorized) to show advertising related to client preferences through the analyses of navigation customer behavior. Continuing navigation will be considered as acceptance of this use. You can change the settings or obtain more information by clicking here. Utilizamos cookies próprios e de terceiros para melhorar nossos serviços e mostrar publicidade relacionada às suas preferências, analisando seus hábitos de navegação. Se continuar a navegar, consideramos que aceita o seu uso. Você pode alterar a configuração ou obter mais informações aqui.