was read the article
array:25 [ "pii" => "S0021755716000528" "issn" => "00217557" "doi" => "10.1016/j.jped.2015.12.004" "estado" => "S300" "fechaPublicacion" => "2016-05-01" "aid" => "368" "copyright" => "Sociedade Brasileira de Pediatria" "copyrightAnyo" => "2016" "documento" => "article" "crossmark" => 1 "licencia" => "http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/" "subdocumento" => "ssu" "cita" => "J Pediatr (Rio J). 2016;92 Supl 1:S14-22" "abierto" => array:3 [ "ES" => true "ES2" => true "LATM" => true ] "gratuito" => true "lecturas" => array:2 [ "total" => 850 "formatos" => array:3 [ "EPUB" => 95 "HTML" => 403 "PDF" => 352 ] ] "Traduccion" => array:1 [ "pt" => array:20 [ "pii" => "S2255553616300088" "issn" => "22555536" "doi" => "10.1016/j.jpedp.2016.03.017" "estado" => "S300" "fechaPublicacion" => "2016-05-01" "aid" => "368" "copyright" => "Sociedade Brasileira de Pediatria" "documento" => "article" "crossmark" => 1 "licencia" => "http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/" "subdocumento" => "ssu" "cita" => "J Pediatr (Rio J). 2016;92 Supl 1:S14-22" "abierto" => array:3 [ "ES" => true "ES2" => true "LATM" => true ] "gratuito" => true "lecturas" => array:2 [ "total" => 1370 "formatos" => array:3 [ "EPUB" => 82 "HTML" => 811 "PDF" => 477 ] ] "pt" => array:12 [ "idiomaDefecto" => true "cabecera" => "<span class="elsevierStyleTextfn">Artigo de revisão</span>" "titulo" => "Behavioral and neural correlates of emotional development: typically developing infants and infants of depressed and/or anxious mothers" "tienePdf" => "pt" "tieneTextoCompleto" => "pt" "tieneResumen" => array:2 [ 0 => "en" 1 => "pt" ] "paginas" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "paginaInicial" => "S14" "paginaFinal" => "S22" ] ] "titulosAlternativos" => array:1 [ "pt" => array:1 [ "titulo" => "Bases neurais e comportamentais do desenvolvimento emocional: lactentes com desenvolvimento típico e lactentes filhos de mães deprimidas e/ou ansiosas" ] ] "contieneResumen" => array:2 [ "en" => true "pt" => true ] "contieneTextoCompleto" => array:1 [ "pt" => true ] "contienePdf" => array:1 [ "pt" => true ] "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "autoresLista" => "Juliana A. Porto, Magda L. Nunes, Charles A. Nelson" "autores" => array:3 [ 0 => array:2 [ "nombre" => "Juliana A." "apellidos" => "Porto" ] 1 => array:2 [ "nombre" => "Magda L." "apellidos" => "Nunes" ] 2 => array:2 [ "nombre" => "Charles A." "apellidos" => "Nelson" ] ] ] ] ] "idiomaDefecto" => "pt" "Traduccion" => array:1 [ "en" => array:9 [ "pii" => "S0021755716000528" "doi" => "10.1016/j.jped.2015.12.004" "estado" => "S300" "subdocumento" => "" "abierto" => array:3 [ "ES" => true "ES2" => true "LATM" => true ] "gratuito" => true "lecturas" => array:1 [ "total" => 0 ] "idiomaDefecto" => "en" "EPUB" => "https://multimedia.elsevier.es/PublicationsMultimediaV1/item/epub/S0021755716000528?idApp=UINPBA000049" ] ] "EPUB" => "https://multimedia.elsevier.es/PublicationsMultimediaV1/item/epub/S2255553616300088?idApp=UINPBA000049" "url" => "/22555536/00000092000003S1/v2_201703310303/S2255553616300088/v2_201703310303/pt/main.assets" ] ] "itemSiguiente" => array:20 [ "pii" => "S0021755716000504" "issn" => "00217557" "doi" => "10.1016/j.jped.2015.12.002" "estado" => "S300" "fechaPublicacion" => "2016-05-01" "aid" => "366" "copyright" => "Sociedade Brasileira de Pediatria" "documento" => "article" "crossmark" => 1 "licencia" => "http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/" "subdocumento" => "ssu" "cita" => "J Pediatr (Rio J). 2016;92 Supl 1:S23-9" "abierto" => array:3 [ "ES" => true "ES2" => true "LATM" => true ] "gratuito" => true "lecturas" => array:2 [ "total" => 717 "formatos" => array:3 [ "EPUB" => 98 "HTML" => 332 "PDF" => 287 ] ] "en" => array:12 [ "idiomaDefecto" => true "cabecera" => "<span class="elsevierStyleTextfn">Review article</span>" "titulo" => "The formation of citizens: the pediatrician's role" "tienePdf" => "en" "tieneTextoCompleto" => "en" "tieneResumen" => array:2 [ 0 => "en" 1 => "pt" ] "paginas" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "paginaInicial" => "S23" "paginaFinal" => "S29" ] ] "titulosAlternativos" => array:1 [ "pt" => array:1 [ "titulo" => "A formação do cidadão: papel do pediatra" ] ] "contieneResumen" => array:2 [ "en" => true "pt" => true ] "contieneTextoCompleto" => array:1 [ "en" => true ] "contienePdf" => array:1 [ "en" => true ] "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "autoresLista" => "Dioclécio Campos Júnior" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "nombre" => "Dioclécio" "apellidos" => "Campos Júnior" ] ] ] ] ] "idiomaDefecto" => "en" "Traduccion" => array:1 [ "pt" => array:9 [ "pii" => "S2255553616300027" "doi" => "10.1016/j.jpedp.2016.03.011" "estado" => "S300" "subdocumento" => "" "abierto" => array:3 [ "ES" => true "ES2" => true "LATM" => true ] "gratuito" => true "lecturas" => array:1 [ "total" => 0 ] "idiomaDefecto" => "pt" "EPUB" => "https://multimedia.elsevier.es/PublicationsMultimediaV1/item/epub/S2255553616300027?idApp=UINPBA000049" ] ] "EPUB" => "https://multimedia.elsevier.es/PublicationsMultimediaV1/item/epub/S0021755716000504?idApp=UINPBA000049" "url" => "/00217557/00000092000003S1/v2_201702240034/S0021755716000504/v2_201702240034/en/main.assets" ] "itemAnterior" => array:20 [ "pii" => "S0021755716000486" "issn" => "00217557" "doi" => "10.1016/j.jped.2016.01.005" "estado" => "S300" "fechaPublicacion" => "2016-05-01" "aid" => "364" "copyright" => "Sociedade Brasileira de Pediatria" "documento" => "article" "crossmark" => 1 "licencia" => "http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/" "subdocumento" => "ssu" "cita" => "J Pediatr (Rio J). 2016;92 Supl 1:S8-S13" "abierto" => array:3 [ "ES" => true "ES2" => true "LATM" => true ] "gratuito" => true "lecturas" => array:2 [ "total" => 1452 "formatos" => array:3 [ "EPUB" => 86 "HTML" => 567 "PDF" => 799 ] ] "en" => array:13 [ "idiomaDefecto" => true "cabecera" => "<span class="elsevierStyleTextfn">Review article</span>" "titulo" => "Language and reading development in the brain today: neuromarkers and the case for prediction" "tienePdf" => "en" "tieneTextoCompleto" => "en" "tieneResumen" => array:2 [ 0 => "en" 1 => "pt" ] "paginas" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "paginaInicial" => "S8" "paginaFinal" => "S13" ] ] "titulosAlternativos" => array:1 [ "pt" => array:1 [ "titulo" => "Desenvolvimento da linguagem e da leitura no cérebro atualmente: neuromarcadores e o caso de predição" ] ] "contieneResumen" => array:2 [ "en" => true "pt" => true ] "contieneTextoCompleto" => array:1 [ "en" => true ] "contienePdf" => array:1 [ "en" => true ] "resumenGrafico" => array:2 [ "original" => 0 "multimedia" => array:7 [ "identificador" => "fig0005" "etiqueta" => "Figure 1" "tipo" => "MULTIMEDIAFIGURA" "mostrarFloat" => true "mostrarDisplay" => false "figura" => array:1 [ 0 => array:4 [ "imagen" => "gr1.jpeg" "Alto" => 622 "Ancho" => 800 "Tamanyo" => 85041 ] ] "descripcion" => array:1 [ "en" => "<p id="spar0045" class="elsevierStyleSimplePara elsevierViewall">The left frontoparietal network of regions for listening and reading (red: listening; green: reading; white: overlap).</p>" ] ] ] "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "autoresLista" => "Augusto Buchweitz" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "nombre" => "Augusto" "apellidos" => "Buchweitz" ] ] ] ] ] "idiomaDefecto" => "en" "Traduccion" => array:1 [ "pt" => array:9 [ "pii" => "S2255553616300040" "doi" => "10.1016/j.jpedp.2016.03.013" "estado" => "S300" "subdocumento" => "" "abierto" => array:3 [ "ES" => true "ES2" => true "LATM" => true ] "gratuito" => true "lecturas" => array:1 [ "total" => 0 ] "idiomaDefecto" => "pt" "EPUB" => "https://multimedia.elsevier.es/PublicationsMultimediaV1/item/epub/S2255553616300040?idApp=UINPBA000049" ] ] "EPUB" => "https://multimedia.elsevier.es/PublicationsMultimediaV1/item/epub/S0021755716000486?idApp=UINPBA000049" "url" => "/00217557/00000092000003S1/v2_201702240034/S0021755716000486/v2_201702240034/en/main.assets" ] "en" => array:19 [ "idiomaDefecto" => true "cabecera" => "<span class="elsevierStyleTextfn">Review article</span>" "titulo" => "Behavioral and neural correlates of emotional development: typically developing infants and infants of depressed and/or anxious mothers" "tieneTextoCompleto" => true "paginas" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "paginaInicial" => "S14" "paginaFinal" => "S22" ] ] "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:4 [ "autoresLista" => "Juliana A. Porto, Magda L. Nunes, Charles A. Nelson" "autores" => array:3 [ 0 => array:4 [ "nombre" => "Juliana" "apellidos" => "A. Porto" "email" => array:1 [ 0 => "juliana.porto@childrens.harvard.edu" ] "referencia" => array:2 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etiqueta" => "<span class="elsevierStyleSup">a</span>" "identificador" => "aff0005" ] 1 => array:2 [ "etiqueta" => "<span class="elsevierStyleSup">*</span>" "identificador" => "cor0005" ] ] ] 1 => array:3 [ "nombre" => "Magda" "apellidos" => "L. Nunes" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etiqueta" => "<span class="elsevierStyleSup">a</span>" "identificador" => "aff0005" ] ] ] 2 => array:3 [ "nombre" => "Charles A." "apellidos" => "Nelson" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etiqueta" => "<span class="elsevierStyleSup">b</span>" "identificador" => "aff0010" ] ] ] ] "afiliaciones" => array:2 [ 0 => array:3 [ "entidad" => "School of Medicine, Pontifícia Universidade Católica do Rio Grande do Sul (PUC-RS), Porto Alegre, RS, Brazil" "etiqueta" => "a" "identificador" => "aff0005" ] 1 => array:3 [ "entidad" => "Laboratories of Cognitive Neuroscience, Division of Developmental Medicine, Boston Children's Hospital, Harvard Medical School (HMS), Boston, United States" "etiqueta" => "b" "identificador" => "aff0010" ] ] "correspondencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "cor0005" "etiqueta" => "⁎" "correspondencia" => "Corresponding author." ] ] ] ] "titulosAlternativos" => array:1 [ "pt" => array:1 [ "titulo" => "Bases neurais e comportamentais do desenvolvimento emocional: lactentes com desenvolvimento típico e lactentes filhos de mães deprimidas e/ou ansiosas" ] ] "textoCompleto" => "<span class="elsevierStyleSections"><span id="sec0005" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0065">Introduction</span><p id="par0005" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">The ability to recognize and understand facial expressions of emotion is a fundamental skill in daily interactions with others, and plays a particularly important role early in life, before the onset of language.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0225"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">1</span></a> Face recognition is one of the most salient cues for social interaction and affective communication. Facial expression recognition develops gradually during infancy and childhood, and appears to continue to develop until early adulthood.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0230"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">2</span></a> During the first year of life, however, the development of visual orientation and the discrimination of different emotions progresses rapidly.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0235"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">3</span></a> Over the past several decades, behavioral studies<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0240"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">4–6</span></a> have used different measures of visual preference to infer aspects of recognition of emotional faces. More recently, new methods to elucidate distinct correlates of brain activation have provided significant contribution to the field. Two of the most used methods, known as electroencephalogram (EEG) and event-related potentials (ERP), are reviewed here.</p><p id="par0010" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Evidence has become available that stress exposure during pregnancy and the postnatal period leads to several long lasting detrimental outcomes in the offspring, including behavior and cognitive problems and neurodevelopmental delay.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0255"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">7</span></a> Studies on maternal negative affective states, including both depression and anxiety, indicate a detrimental effect on the child's health and development, increasing the risk for a wide range of disorders such as low birth weight and preterm birth, cognitive and motor developmental delay, achievement deficits, and psychiatric disorders.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0260"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">8</span></a> Infants of depressed and anxious mothers have increased vulnerability to cognitive and emotional problems throughout their lifespan.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0255"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">7,8</span></a></p><p id="par0015" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Psychological stress, depression, and anxiety are closely linked and often coexist.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0255"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">7</span></a> Approximately 10–20% of women will exhibit symptoms of depression during pregnancy and/or the postpartum period.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0255"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">7</span></a> Anxiety disorders in the perinatal period have received more scientific attention only recently and their prevalence is still unclear, yet estimates range as high as 30%.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0265"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">9</span></a> The outcomes of anxiety and depression are often studied together, as the symptoms frequently overlap, and their coexistence is a marker of severity.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0265"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">9,10</span></a></p><p id="par0020" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">The mechanisms between maternal negative affective states and the infant outcomes are studied both in animal and human clinical research. During pregnancy, maternal stress induces the dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) system, elevating the cortisol levels and inducing sympathetic activation with release of catecholamines.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0255"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">7,9</span></a> The latter is associated to increased uterine artery resistance, reducing the blood flow to the fetus, with restricted inflow of oxygen and nutrients.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0270"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">10,11</span></a></p><p id="par0025" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">The higher levels of maternal cortisol adversely affect fetal brain development, possibly due to epigenetic dysregulation through alterations in synaptogenesis and neurotransmitter functions.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0275"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">11,12</span></a> There is evidence of disruption of the fetal HPA system, with adverse physiological and biochemical effects on the fetus and newborn<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0270"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">10</span></a> that can persist throughout infancy, resulting in altered infant perception and behavior.</p><p id="par0030" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">In the postnatal period, maternal anxiety and depression are related to less sensitive and inconsistent care when interacting with infants, providing suboptimal levels of general stimulation, and disrupting the mother–child relationship and the formation of attachments.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0260"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">8,13</span></a> Accumulating evidence indicates that the emotional environment of the infants’ daily experiences influences their developmental trajectory of facial recognition.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0290"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">14</span></a> Typically, the mother is the most present person in an infant's life, and the mother's facial expressions are the most prevalent in their experience.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0290"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">14</span></a> Mothers with depression and anxiety smile less, display more flat affect and negative facial expressions, and interact with infants in a withdrawn and muted style.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0290"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">14,15</span></a> As a result, infants of depressed and/or anxious mothers have systematically atypical social experiences compared to infants of healthy mothers.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0290"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">14,16</span></a> Understanding the possible mechanisms by which maternal stress related to depression and anxiety affects an infant's development is a key to further developing better intervention strategies and prevention programs.</p><p id="par0035" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">This review examines the findings of studies about maternal depression and anxiety on face recognition by the infant, both regarding behavior and neurophysiology. It begins with a brief review of typically developing infants in order to set the stage for the discussion that follows, focused on infants of depressed and/or anxious mothers.</p><span id="sec0010" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0070">Behavioral studies</span><p id="par0040" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">The majority of early emotional behavioral studies measure looking time or visual preference. Visual fixation gradually decreases to a repeatedly presented stimulus, a phenomenon referred to as habituation (or familiarization); presentation of a new stimulus leads to recovery of looking if the infants can discriminate the old from the new stimulus (dishabituation).<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0235"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">3</span></a> This measure can be used to examine the ability to discriminate different visual stimuli, such as one face from another or one object from another, or in the current context, one facial expression from another. Another method to measure visual preference is the visual paired comparison procedure, in which looking time and duration of the first visual fixation are measured comparing two expressions seen at the same time.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0235"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">3</span></a></p><p id="par0045" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Emotional information can also be inferred from multisensory modalities presented simultaneously, such as facial and vocal stimuli. Multimodal studies investigate how different sensory stimuli may influence the processing and perception of each other.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0245"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">5,17</span></a> For example, studies analyzing congruent and incongruent face–voice pairs (<span class="elsevierStyleItalic">i.e.</span>, same or different emotion presented by the face–voice) attempt to elucidate how emotional information is integrated.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0245"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">5</span></a></p><p id="par0050" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Furthermore, emotional responses are measured observing infants’ specific behavioral reactions, such as facial expressions, vocalization, imitation, or body movements. One commonly used paradigm is the still-face, in which the mother (or the experimenter) is instructed to show flat affect, mimicking emotional unavailability. Infants typically respond with distress, manifested by less motor activity, frowning, gaze aversion and crying.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0285"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">13,18,19</span></a></p></span><span id="sec0015" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0075">Neural correlates - Electroencephalogram (EEG)</span><p id="par0055" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">The EEG is a measure of electrophysiological brain activity that represents synchronized activation of large populations of cortical pyramidal neurons firing together. The synchronization of electrical activity generates different continuous frequencies of oscillation, measurable by electrodes placed at the scalp. EEG is a non-invasive method that can be used in infant-friendly environments and has an excellent temporal resolution.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0320"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">20</span></a> The study field of emotional development has especially explored the pattern of frontal EEG power in the alpha frequency.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0320"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">20,21</span></a> Alpha frequency appears early in life, matures rapidly over the first few years, and thereafter remains relatively stable.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0330"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">22</span></a> Alpha power is inversely related to brain activity, confirmed with hemodynamic and metabolic measures (<span class="elsevierStyleItalic">i.e.</span>, negatively correlated with cerebral perfusion in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and with cerebral glucose metabolism using positron emission tomography (PET)).<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0300"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">16,21</span></a> Therefore, EEG alpha power is used as an inverse indicator of regional cortical activation. The EEG frontal asymmetry (FA) is a measure computing the difference between the scores of alpha power comparing frontal right and left areas.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0325"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">21,23</span></a></p><p id="par0060" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">A large body of empirical work measuring FA relates different patterns of activation to differentially specialized types of emotions.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0320"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">20,21</span></a> Greater relative left FA is associated with approach behaviors (such as joy, anger, and surgency) and with the expression of positive affect. In turn, greater relative right FA is associated with avoidance and withdrawal behaviors, as well as the expression of certain negative emotions, such as fear and sadness. The studies<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0300"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">16,21–24</span></a> of FA are related to both trait and state measures, considering individual differences in affective style and emotional disorders, and acute affective response, respectively.</p></span><span id="sec0020" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0080">Neural correlates - Event-related potentials (ERPs)</span><p id="par0065" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">ERPs are the most common method used in infancy to investigate the neural correlates of a variety of perceptual and cognitive functions. ERPs are transient changes in brain activity that occur in response to a discrete event, extracted from the EEG recording. Electrical brain activity is measured during the presentation of repeated stimuli, revealing reliable patterns according to each stimuli category. It is successfully used to investigate perception discrimination, emotion recognition, and memory in infants and adults.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0345"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">25</span></a></p><p id="par0070" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Studies in infants have identified several components involved in visual human face processing: P1 (positive deflection that peaks around 120<span class="elsevierStyleHsp" style=""></span>ms after stimulus onset), N290 (negative deflection at 290<span class="elsevierStyleHsp" style=""></span>ms post stimulus), P400 (positive deflection at 390–450<span class="elsevierStyleHsp" style=""></span>ms for infants between 3 and 12 months of age), Nc (Negative central deflection peaking around 400<span class="elsevierStyleHsp" style=""></span>ms after stimulus), and positive slow wave (PSW, positive deflection beginning around 800<span class="elsevierStyleHsp" style=""></span>ms after stimulus).<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0345"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">25,26</span></a> The precise meaning of each component and the developmental trajectory remain to be clarified both in children and in adults, although a number of recent studies have begun to shed light on this subject. Specifically, consistent evidence relates similar face-sensitive processing for both the N290 and the P400 to the N170, a component reliably studied in face processing in adults.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0345"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">25</span></a> Additionally, the Nc is considered an index of attention and orienting to salient stimuli in infants, stimuli that recruit more attention appear to enhance the amplitude of the Nc.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0355"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">27</span></a></p><p id="par0075" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">The electrophysiological processes underlying multimodal sensory integration in emotion began recently to be examined throughout development. Auditory ERP components can be explored in paradigms using simultaneous face–voice stimuli.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0245"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">5</span></a> Some authors have shown that the previously described Nc and a positive component (Pc, a component with similar characteristics of the PSW, possibly equivalent to it) are also sensitive in crossmodal face–voice stimuli. The enhanced amplitude of the Nc is related to attention orienting to unexpected/unfamiliar stimuli, while the Pc evinces a larger amplitude to familiar stimuli.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0305"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">17</span></a> Other researchers refer to infants’ responses to auditory stimuli as the P150-N250-P350-N450 ERP complex, and consider the N450 equivalent to the Nc.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0360"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">28</span></a> These infant components are believed to be precursors of children's and adults’ components (P1, N1, N2, P2, P3a, and N4) and can already be observed at birth.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0360"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">28</span></a></p></span><span id="sec0025" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0085">Typically developing infants</span><p id="par0080" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Studies with newborns reveal that infants already look longer and preferentially orient to face-like stimuli several hours after birth,<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0365"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">29</span></a> suggesting they have some ability to orient to the most salient social stimuli in their environment: faces. The developmental process is defined as “experience expectant”: the innate neural architecture has the potential to become specialized for face processing, but it needs to be primed through experience, allowing the face-processing pathway to mature.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0350"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">26</span></a></p><p id="par0085" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">A few studies<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0370"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">30–32</span></a> have shown that newborns already react distinctly to different facial expressions. Field et al.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0370"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">30,31</span></a> conducted a series of studies with newborns, both term and preterm, using dishabituation procedure and behavior observation. The authors reported that newborns were able to discriminate and imitate happy, surprise, and sad expressions (although there was no control group in this study). In a more recent dishabituation study, term newborns showed increased looking time to happy compared to fearful faces presented at the same time, and no difference between neutral and fearful categories.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0380"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">32</span></a></p><p id="par0090" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Even though there is evidence that newborns might differentiate some facial expressions, it is not until 3–4 months of age that infants can reliably distinguish among happy compared to some other emotional expressions.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0245"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">5</span></a> Infants at 3 months of age can discriminate among happy and surprised faces,<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0245"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">5</span></a> and between happy and sad faces of their own mothers or a stranger.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0250"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">6</span></a> In another experiment,<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0240"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">4</span></a> 3-month-olds discriminated between happy and neutral faces and within the positive emotional category (<span class="elsevierStyleItalic">i.e.</span>, different degrees of happy), demonstrating increasing positive visual preference with the intensity of the smile rising, peaking with maximally toothy smiles. In this study, maternal style to the infants’ perceptual sensitivity was categorized. Mothers who actively encouraged their infants to attend to them more often had infants who detected facial expressions of smiling more readily, a possible reflection of the effects of early experience with the mother's interaction styles.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0240"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">4</span></a></p><p id="par0095" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Accordingly, happy faces appear to be the first to be discriminated in infants comparing to all other facial expressions.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0365"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">29</span></a> The infant's early preference for positive emotions is believed to be related to the necessity of bonding at this attachment formation stage.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0365"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">29</span></a> As the infant pays more attention, the caregiver smiles and the infant imitates it back, promoting a positive environment and strengthening their relationship, crucial to the infant's survival.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0240"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">4,29</span></a></p><p id="par0100" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">The studies on infants after the age of 7 months present more consistent data of the infant's ability to categorize other expressions than happiness.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0245"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">5</span></a> In fact, at some point between 5 and 7 months of age, infants develop a preference for fearful faces over other emotions.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0225"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">1</span></a> Seven-month-old infants look longer to fearful than to neutral or happy faces, and are less likely to disengage attention from fearful faces.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0225"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">1,33</span></a> This pattern is typically seen in adults, possibly to prioritize the identification of potential environmental threats.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0225"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">1</span></a> As infants begin to crawl, and their locomotion ability improves this response, thus may reflect an adaptive increase in vigilance in response to cues of threat in the environment.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0225"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">1,33</span></a></p><p id="par0105" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">In a multimodal face–voice experiment, 5-month-old infants reliably detected emotional vocal changes, but only if there was a simultaneous presentation of faces, suggesting that facial cues might facilitate infants’ perception of emotional voice tones.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0390"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">34</span></a> In another study, 7-month-old infants recognized face–voice common affect, displaying preference for face and voice emotionally congruent matching stimuli rather than incongruent ones, even when voice was played out of synchrony with the face.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0245"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">5</span></a></p></span><span id="sec0030" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0090">Neural correlates</span><p id="par0110" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Stable patterns of FA emerge early in life and are related to individual differences in emotional trait dispositions, such as emotion regulation and reactivity.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0300"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">16,20,21</span></a> Infants with more difficult temperaments (<span class="elsevierStyleItalic">i.e.</span>, highly reactive, fearful, and inhibited) show greater relative right FA.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0255"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">7,10</span></a> FA is also observed in acute affective response, indicating a current emotional state. Fox and Davidson<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0395"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">35</span></a> conducted a series of studies examining FA during a variety of elicitors producing positive or negative emotions. Newborns were tested with water, sucrose, and citric acid solutions while concomitant EEG was recorded. The solutions elicited facial expressions that the experimenters coded as interest and disgust, which were associated to greater left FA in response to a pleasant taste (sucrose) compared to unpleasant (citric acid) or neutral tastes.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0395"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">35</span></a></p><p id="par0115" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Typically developing 10-month-old infants watched videotaped segments of a female model displaying happy or sad facial expressions. The infants showed greater relative left FA when observing the happy expressions.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0400"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">36</span></a> In another study,<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0335"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">23</span></a> the same authors documented asymmetrical EEG activation in the frontal cortex when 10-month-old infants were observed while exhibiting specific behaviors. Infants showed greater left FA when expressing approach behaviors like reaching with hands for their mother and eliciting facial expressions of joy accompanied by positive vocalizations. When the same infants displayed behaviors such as gaze aversion and distress (<span class="elsevierStyleItalic">i.e.</span>, active withdrawal behavior), there was greater relative right FA.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0335"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">23</span></a> In a similar posterior study, 6-month-old infants who demonstrated fear and sadness (while a stranger was approaching) had greater right FA.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0405"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">37</span></a></p><p id="par0120" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">In the first study in infants using ERPs to analyze differences in response to facial emotional states, Nelson and De Haan<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0355"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">27</span></a> reported that 7-month-old infants evinced a larger Nc when seeing fearful instead of happy faces, and showed no differences when tested only on negative emotions (angry and fear). The authors also reported variations on the amplitude of two positive components, an early and a later positive component, both of greater amplitude to happy rather than to fearful faces. Additional studies corroborate with the finding of enhanced Nc amplitude and attentional response to emotional face stimuli.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0225"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">1,23,33</span></a></p><p id="par0125" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">In a multimodal processing study, 7-month-old infants demonstrated a larger Nc for emotionally incongruent face–voice pairs of happy and angry stimuli, whereas congruent stimuli elicited larger amplitude for the Pc.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0305"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">17</span></a> The pattern of an attenuated Nc and a larger Pc was related to the recognition of the congruent pairing.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0305"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">17</span></a> Recently, 9-month-old infants revealed modified auditory ERP components (larger positivity on P150 and P350 and smaller negativity in N250 and N450) for either happy or fearful vocalizations when preceded by visual exposition to fearful faces.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0410"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">38</span></a> Both P150 and P350 are related to orienting attention; therefore, the authors concluded that fearful faces enhanced attentional levels, modulating ERP responses.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0410"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">38</span></a></p></span><span id="sec0035" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0095">Infants of depressed and anxious mothers</span><p id="par0130" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Maternal depression and anxiety are implicated in atypical behaviors in infants since birth.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0265"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">9</span></a> Infants of depressed and anxious mothers are believed to have higher arousal and less attentiveness,<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0280"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">12</span></a> showing less orientation to facial expressions and face–voice pairs in experimental conditions and in live face–voice interactions.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0295"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">15</span></a> Studies of behavior and neural correlates are beginning to improve insights into the mechanisms underlying the presumably slower sensory processing and delayed attention in these infants.</p><p id="par0135" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Most of the studies on maternal negative affective states are based on symptoms scales rather than a confirmed diagnosis of maternal depression and anxiety. Although self-report scales do not provide a clinical diagnosis, they have been correlated with confirmed diagnoses on clinical evaluations.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0260"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">8,9,22</span></a> It is particularly noteworthy to emphasize that the use of depression symptoms scales has been shown to invariably assess a wide range of anxiety symptoms, as well as other negative affective states such as anger and irritability.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0270"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">10,11</span></a> Therefore, although the majority of studies reviewed here rely on depression symptoms scales, it is believed that the related outcomes may be secondary to maternal anxiety aspects as well.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0260"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">8–10</span></a></p><p id="par0140" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Studies on newborns indicate that infants of depressed mothers (IDMs) orient less to faces and voices as early as during the first hours of life. Hernandez-Reif et al.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0415"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">39</span></a> tested full-term newborns of depressed and non-depressed mothers for visual preference and habituation to the mother's face–voice, comparing to a female stranger. IDMs required one-third more trials and almost twice as long as the infants of nondepressed mothers (INDMs) to habituate to their mother's face–voice pairing. In the post-test visual preference phase, IDMs failed to discriminate their mothers from a stranger. In a subsequent experiment,<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0420"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">40</span></a> a group of mothers were evaluated longitudinally for continuity of depressive symptoms pre- and postnatally, including comorbid anxiety. Their 3-month-old infants were exposed to video clips of female models with face and voice stimuli for happy and sad conditions. As earlier reported in newborns, IDMs required longer time to habituate to faces, particularly to happy facial expressions. Unexpectedly, IDMs were able to discriminate sad from happy expressions but only if they were first habituated to sad, thereby indicating that they may not perceive sad expressions as a novelty.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0420"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">40</span></a></p><p id="par0145" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Using a still-face procedure, 3-month-old IDMs exhibited less distress and fewer negative expressions comparing to the typical response of INDMs, possibly related to being more accustomed to a less expressive environment characterized by their mothers’ relatively flat affect and less interactive behaviors.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0310"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">18,19</span></a> Beyond that, IDMs had a less interactive behavior (<span class="elsevierStyleItalic">i.e.</span>, fewer positive and negative behaviors) during the recorded spontaneous mother-infant interactions, when mothers were instructed to engage with their infant in play, as they would usually do at home.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0315"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">19</span></a> In a study<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0285"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">13</span></a> that added a tactile component to the still-face procedure, mothers were asked to maintain a neutral face while touching the infant. Three-month-old IDMs showed more positive affect, manifesting more smiles and vocalizations than infants in the still-face control group without touching. The authors suggest that providing touch stimulation can increase infants’ attention and positive affect, thus improving the interactions of depressed mothers and their infants.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0285"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">13</span></a></p><p id="par0150" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Five-month-old infants of mothers with a confirmed diagnosis of depression were successfully habituated to neutral or smiling faces, but they later failed to discriminate between the facial expressions.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0290"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">14</span></a> Striano et al.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0425"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">41</span></a> investigated 6-month-old IDMs and INDMs and found that all infants were able to discriminate neutral from progressively higher intensities of smiling and frowning faces. IDMs, however, presented a looking preference for all smiling faces, and a greater preferential looking to high intensity smiling and frowning expressions, a pattern not observed in INDMs.</p><p id="par0155" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Hence, there is evidence that IDMs show less interest in faces, orient poorly to synchronized visual and vocal stimuli, and have diminished sensitivity to changes in facial expressions from birth and throughout the first months of life.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0295"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">15</span></a> The abnormalities are speculated to be related to deficits in attentiveness, as well as altered perceptual skills, perhaps secondary to atypical visual and/or auditory sensory processing.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0290"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">14,15</span></a></p></span><span id="sec0040" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0100">Neural correlates</span><p id="par0160" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">There is consistent evidence that IDMs show greater relative right FA than do infants whose mothers are not depressed. This pattern is reported from newborns studies, remaining stable throughout infancy up to childhood.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0300"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">16,20</span></a> The right FA bias is also exhibited by depressed adults, which remains stable even after a remission of the depressive symptoms.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0330"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">22</span></a> Relative right FA is also associated with anxiety.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0275"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">11</span></a></p><p id="par0165" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">During face-to-face interactions, 3-month-old IDMs compared to INDMs were less responsive to facial expressions, looked longer at sad faces, and displayed less positive and more often negative faces themselves.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0430"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">42</span></a> Another group of 3-month-old watched videos of a female model displaying happy and sad facial and vocal expressions. The INDMs exhibited greater relative right FA when viewing sad compared to happy face–voice stimuli. No differences on FA were found for IDMs, possibly because the EEG data were analyzed during the whole experiment, rather than accounting for only the periods when the infants were actually looking at the videos.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0435"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">43</span></a> In a subsequent study, the same researchers analyzed 3–6-month-old IDMs’ and INDMs’ EEG responses to mothers’ and strangers’ happy, surprised, and sad facial expressions. This time, the researchers only computed the EEG data for the periods when the infants were attending to facial expressions. Both groups of infants showed greater right FA during their mothers’ and strangers’ sad <span class="elsevierStyleItalic">vs.</span> happy expressions; however, IDMs had significantly greater FA compared to INDMs throughout the different expressions of both the mothers and strangers. IDMs were less interested in facial expressions, showed less positive and more negative affect, and evinced increased salivary cortisol levels after the experiment.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0340"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">24</span></a></p><p id="par0170" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Diego et al.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0440"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">44</span></a> further investigated infants of mothers with two different styles of depressive behavior, intrusive and withdrawn. Accordingly, the interaction style and the biochemical profiles differ between the two styles. The withdrawn mothers tended to have more flat affect, less frequent vocalizing as well as touching, and lower dopamine levels. Their infants also showed lower dopamine levels and displayed greater relative right FA. Intrusive mothers showed rough physical contact and quick and loud verbal behavior when interacting with their infants. In the study, 3-month-old infants observed their own mother and a stranger, in happy, surprised, and sad expressions. Infants of intrusive mothers looked longer to surprise and sad expressions compared to happy ones performed by a stranger, and displayed a concomitant greater relative right FA activity. These infants also had an increased salivary cortisol, possibly reflecting a higher response to the stressful stimulus. The nature of the different maternal depressive styles and how they affect the infants’ physiology and behavior have not yet been fully understood and require further research.</p><p id="par0175" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">As previously reviewed, ERP provide an excellent tool to temporarily correlate behavior and neural response and is easily administered throughout development.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0345"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">25</span></a> A growing number of studies have been performed in children and adults, expanding the research on neural vulnerability markers for psychopathological disorders, including depression and anxiety.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0345"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">25</span></a> In infancy, a few studies on auditory ERP and maternal affective disorders have begun to demonstrate neural processing alterations, providing insights into the underlying developmental pathways that remain to be better clarified.</p><p id="par0180" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">In a recent study using ERP and multimodal processing, Otte et al.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0280"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">12</span></a> analyzed 9-month-old infants exposed to maternal anxiety during pregnancy (10% of the mothers also reported previous treatment for depression). Infants were presented with happy and fearful facial and vocal stimuli. Infants prenatally exposed to higher levels of anxiety exhibited significantly larger P350 amplitudes and a trend for larger P150 amplitudes after fearful vocalizations, regardless of the preceding visual emotion type, potentially related to an increased attention to fearful vocal stimuli. The findings corroborate studies in children and adults that anxiety symptoms heightened the sensitivity to threat-related information.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0280"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">12</span></a></p><p id="par0185" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">The findings of studies on infants of depressed and anxious mothers are summarized in <a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#tbl0005">Table 1</a>.</p><elsevierMultimedia ident="tbl0005"></elsevierMultimedia></span></span><span id="sec0045" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0105">Conclusions</span><p id="par0190" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">In summary, infants of depressed and anxious mothers have increased risk for several detrimental outcomes across the lifespan. They exhibit more difficult temperaments (<span class="elsevierStyleItalic">i.e.</span>, highly reactive, fearful, and inhibited) and higher incidence of attentional, emotional, and behavioral problems – such as depression, anxiety, and conduct disorders<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0265"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">9,10</span></a> – throughout childhood, adolescence, and adulthood.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRefs" href="#bib0255"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">7,9,10</span></a></p><p id="par0195" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">During the first year of life, these infants display a number of atypical behaviors, including less interest in facial expressions, less smiling and vocalizations, more time required to habituate to faces and to face–voice pairs, and failure to discriminate between different emotions.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0295"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">15</span></a> Impairments in sensory and perceptual processing as well as reduced attentiveness may underlie such behaviors. Recent neurophysiological studies have begun to shed light on the possible mechanisms linking maternal depression and anxiety to outcomes in infants. EEG studies specifically analyzing facial emotion recognition corroborate findings from the extant literature of a relative right FA asymmetry and its association with depression and negative effects. Comparing to INDMs, IDMs show significantly greater relative right FA across different emotional expressions.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0340"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">24</span></a> A recent unique ERP study described a correlation between maternal anxiety and infants’ enhanced ERP components to fearful stimuli, potentially related to an increased bias to threat in these infants.<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0280"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">12</span></a></p><p id="par0200" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Further research is needed to better clarify the potential mechanisms related to infants’ negative outcomes. Expanding the research with current behavior and neurophysiological methods, as well as exploring new tools such as near-infrared spectroscopy, can help detect biologically-based markers that may mediate these associations from the earliest stages of life, months and years prior to adverse clinical outcomes. Fostering clinical and research integration, by incorporating investigation tools in clinical practice or promoting longitudinal studies in risk populations, for example, should facilitate studying individual differences throughout development and enable the potential identification of precocious neural changes in infants associated with the later onset of clinical symptoms. Beyond that, developmental research might eventually contribute to refining screening tools, improving treatment and enabling primary prevention interventions for children at risk.</p></span><span id="sec0050" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0110">Funding</span><p id="par0205" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">Juliana A. Porto is supported by <span class="elsevierStyleGrantSponsor" id="gs1">CAPES/PDSE</span> at PUCRS and by a research fellowship at the Laboratories of Cognitive Neuroscience, Boston Children's Hospital/Harvard Medical School. Magda L. Nunes is a PQ researcher from CNPq Brazil.</p></span><span id="sec0055" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0115">Conflicts of interest</span><p id="par0210" class="elsevierStylePara elsevierViewall">The authors declare no conflicts of interest.</p></span></span>" "textoCompletoSecciones" => array:1 [ "secciones" => array:9 [ 0 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "xres805569" "titulo" => "Abstract" "secciones" => array:4 [ 0 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0005" "titulo" => "Objectives" ] 1 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0010" "titulo" => "Sources" ] 2 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0015" "titulo" => "Summary of the findings" ] 3 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0020" "titulo" => "Conclusions" ] ] ] 1 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "xpalclavsec803687" "titulo" => "Keywords" ] 2 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "xres805568" "titulo" => "Resumo" "secciones" => array:4 [ 0 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0025" "titulo" => "Objetivos" ] 1 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0030" "titulo" => "Fontes" ] 2 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0035" "titulo" => "Resumo dos achados" ] 3 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0040" "titulo" => "Conclusões" ] ] ] 3 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "xpalclavsec803688" "titulo" => "Palavras-chave" ] 4 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "sec0005" "titulo" => "Introduction" "secciones" => array:7 [ 0 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "sec0010" "titulo" => "Behavioral studies" ] 1 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "sec0015" "titulo" => "Neural correlates - Electroencephalogram (EEG)" ] 2 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "sec0020" "titulo" => "Neural correlates - Event-related potentials (ERPs)" ] 3 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "sec0025" "titulo" => "Typically developing infants" ] 4 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "sec0030" "titulo" => "Neural correlates" ] 5 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "sec0035" "titulo" => "Infants of depressed and anxious mothers" ] 6 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "sec0040" "titulo" => "Neural correlates" ] ] ] 5 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "sec0045" "titulo" => "Conclusions" ] 6 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "sec0050" "titulo" => "Funding" ] 7 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "sec0055" "titulo" => "Conflicts of interest" ] 8 => array:1 [ "titulo" => "References" ] ] ] "pdfFichero" => "main.pdf" "tienePdf" => true "fechaRecibido" => "2015-11-12" "fechaAceptado" => "2015-11-25" "PalabrasClave" => array:2 [ "en" => array:1 [ 0 => array:4 [ "clase" => "keyword" "titulo" => "Keywords" "identificador" => "xpalclavsec803687" "palabras" => array:6 [ 0 => "Infant" 1 => "Depressed mothers" 2 => "Anxiety" 3 => "Face" 4 => "Emotion" 5 => "Behavior" ] ] ] "pt" => array:1 [ 0 => array:4 [ "clase" => "keyword" "titulo" => "Palavras-chave" "identificador" => "xpalclavsec803688" "palabras" => array:6 [ 0 => "Lactente" 1 => "Mães Deprimidas" 2 => "Ansiedade" 3 => "Face" 4 => "Emoção" 5 => "Comportamento" ] ] ] ] "tieneResumen" => true "resumen" => array:2 [ "en" => array:3 [ "titulo" => "Abstract" "resumen" => "<span id="abst0005" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0010">Objectives</span><p id="spar0005" class="elsevierStyleSimplePara elsevierViewall">To describe the main findings of studies of behavioral and neural correlates regarding the development of facial emotion processing during the first year of life in typically developing infants and infants of depressed and/or anxious mothers.</p></span> <span id="abst0010" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0015">Sources</span><p id="spar0010" class="elsevierStyleSimplePara elsevierViewall">Comprehensive, non-systematic review of the literature on studies about individual differences in facial emotion processing by newborns and infants over the first year of life.</p></span> <span id="abst0015" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0020">Summary of the findings</span><p id="spar0015" class="elsevierStyleSimplePara elsevierViewall">Maternal stress related to depression and anxiety has been associated to atypical emotional processing and attentional behaviors in the offspring. Recent neurophysiological studies using electroencephalogram and event-related potentials have begun to shed light on the possible mechanisms underlying such behaviors.</p></span> <span id="abst0020" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0025">Conclusions</span><p id="spar0020" class="elsevierStyleSimplePara elsevierViewall">Infants of depressed and/or anxious mothers have increased risk for several adverse outcomes across the lifespan. Further neurobehavioral investigations and the promotion of clinical and developmental research integration might eventually contribute to refining screening tools, improving treatment, and enabling primary prevention interventions for children at risk.</p></span>" "secciones" => array:4 [ 0 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0005" "titulo" => "Objectives" ] 1 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0010" "titulo" => "Sources" ] 2 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0015" "titulo" => "Summary of the findings" ] 3 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0020" "titulo" => "Conclusions" ] ] ] "pt" => array:3 [ "titulo" => "Resumo" "resumen" => "<span id="abst0025" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0035">Objetivos</span><p id="spar0025" class="elsevierStyleSimplePara elsevierViewall">Descrever os principais achados de estudos de correlação entre o comportamento e as bases neurais em relação ao processamento de emoções faciais durante o primeiro ano de vida de lactentes com desenvolvimento típico e lactentes de mães deprimidas e/ou ansiosas.</p></span> <span id="abst0030" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0040">Fontes</span><p id="spar0030" class="elsevierStyleSimplePara elsevierViewall">Análise abrangente e não sistemática da literatura de estudos sobre diferenças individuais no processamento de emoções faciais de neonatos e lactentes ao longo do primeiro ano de vida.</p></span> <span id="abst0035" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0045">Resumo dos achados</span><p id="spar0035" class="elsevierStyleSimplePara elsevierViewall">O estresse materno relacionado à depressão e ansiedade tem sido associado a alterações no processamento emocional e na alocação da atenção da prole. Estudos neurofisiológicos recentes utilizando electroencefalograma e potenciais relacionados a eventos começam a esclarecer os possíveis mecanismos inerentes a esses comportamentos.</p></span> <span id="abst0040" class="elsevierStyleSection elsevierViewall"><span class="elsevierStyleSectionTitle" id="sect0050">Conclusões</span><p id="spar0040" class="elsevierStyleSimplePara elsevierViewall">Lactentes filhos de mães deprimidas e/ou ansiosas têm maior risco de problemas de saúde física e mental durante toda vida. O avanço de estudos neurocomportamentais e a promoção de integração entre a pesquisa clínica e de desenvolvimento poderão contribuir para refinar as ferramentas de triagem, melhorar o tratamento e permitir intervenções de prevenção primária para crianças em risco.</p></span>" "secciones" => array:4 [ 0 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0025" "titulo" => "Objetivos" ] 1 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0030" "titulo" => "Fontes" ] 2 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0035" "titulo" => "Resumo dos achados" ] 3 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "abst0040" "titulo" => "Conclusões" ] ] ] ] "NotaPie" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etiqueta" => "☆" "nota" => "<p class="elsevierStyleNotepara" id="npar0010">Please cite this article as: Porto JA, Nunes ML, Nelson CA. Behavioral and neural correlates of emotional development: typically developing infants and infants of depressed and/or anxious mothers. J Pediatr (Rio J). 2016;92(3 Suppl 1):S14–22.</p>" ] ] "multimedia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:8 [ "identificador" => "tbl0005" "etiqueta" => "Table 1" "tipo" => "MULTIMEDIATABLA" "mostrarFloat" => true "mostrarDisplay" => false "detalles" => array:1 [ 0 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "at1" "detalle" => "Table " "rol" => "short" ] ] "tabla" => array:3 [ "leyenda" => "<p id="spar0050" class="elsevierStyleSimplePara elsevierViewall">h, hours; mo, months; IDMs, infants of depressed mothers; INDMs, infants of non-depressed mothers; EEG, electroencephalogram; ERPs, event-related potentials; FA, frontal asymmetry.</p>" "tablatextoimagen" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "tabla" => array:1 [ 0 => """ <table border="0" frame="\n \t\t\t\t\tvoid\n \t\t\t\t" class=""><thead title="thead"><tr title="table-row"><th class="td" title="table-head " align="left" valign="top" scope="col" style="border-bottom: 2px solid black">Authors \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</th><th class="td" title="table-head " align="left" valign="top" scope="col" style="border-bottom: 2px solid black">Participants (<span class="elsevierStyleItalic">n</span>) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</th><th class="td" title="table-head " align="left" valign="top" scope="col" style="border-bottom: 2px solid black">Mean age<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#tblfn0005"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">a</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</th><th class="td" title="table-head " align="left" valign="top" scope="col" style="border-bottom: 2px solid black">Method \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</th><th class="td" title="table-head " align="left" valign="top" scope="col" style="border-bottom: 2px solid black">Findings/results \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</th></tr></thead><tbody title="tbody"><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Hernandez-Reif et al., 2002<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0415"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">39</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">20 (10 IDMs) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">45<span class="elsevierStyleHsp" style=""></span>h \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Habituation and dishabituation to mother and stranger face–voice \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">IDMs required more trials and took longer to habituate to their mothers’ face and voice<br>IDMs failed to discriminate their own mothers from a stranger \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Field, 1984<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0310"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">18</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">28 (14 IDMs) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">3.0 mo \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Mother-infant interaction, still-face procedure, and infant behavior coding \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">IDMs showed less distress in still-face procedure (less negative facial expressions and less vocalizations) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Pickens & Field, 1995<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0430"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">42</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">84 (27 IDMs) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">3.1 mo \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Mother-infant interaction and infant facial expression coding \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">IDMs displayed more sad and anger expressions and less interest expressions \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Diego et al., 2002<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0440"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">44</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">27 (intrusive<span class="elsevierStyleHsp" style=""></span>=<span class="elsevierStyleHsp" style=""></span>14, withdrawn<span class="elsevierStyleHsp" style=""></span>=<span class="elsevierStyleHsp" style=""></span>13) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">3.3 mo \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Mother-infant interaction, visual preference for mother and stranger facial expressions<br>Infant behavior coding<br>EEG recorded during visual preference \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Infants of intrusive mothers looked longer to surprise and sad than to happy expressions performed by a stranger, displaying a concomitant greater relative right FA activity \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Peláez-Nogueras et al., 1996<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0285"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">13</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">48 (24 IDMs) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">3.4 mo \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Mother-infant interaction, still-face and still-face-with-touch procedures \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">IDMs displayed less distress and negative behaviors in still-face procedure<br>IDMs submitted to still-face-with-touch procedure showed more positive affect (more smiles and vocalizations) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Hernandez-Reif et al., 2006<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0420"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">40</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">32 (16 IDMs) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">3.5 mo \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Habituation and dishabituation to face–voice \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">IDMs required longer time to habituate to faces, particularly to happy faces \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Field et al., 1998<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0435"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">43</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">24 (12 IDMs) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">3.7 mo \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Visual preference for face–voice<br>Infant facial expression coding<br>EEG recorded during visual preference \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">INDMs looked longer to sad face–voice stimuli than IDM, and exhibited greater relative right FA while exposed to sad comparing to happy stimuli. No differences on FA were found for IDM \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Field et al., 2007<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0315"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">19</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">28 (14 IDMs) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">4 mo \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Mother-infant interaction, still-face procedure, and infant behavior coding \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">IDMs showed less smiling and vocalizing, more gaze aversion and motor activity during mother-infant interaction.<br>IDMs showed less motor activity and less distress behavior (less gaze aversion, distress brow and crying) during still-face procedure \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Diego et al., 2004<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0340"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">24</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">60 (30 IDMs) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">4.2 mo \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Visual preference for mother and stranger facial expressions<br>Infant behavior coding<br>EEG recorded during visual preference \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">IDMs exhibited less positive affect and looked less at their mothers’ surprise and sad expressions. IDM looked less at all strangers’ facial expressions, showed less positive affect during happy and surprise and more negative affect during surprise expressions<br>IDMs had significantly greater relative FA compared to INDMs in all different expressions of both the mothers and strangers<br>IDMs and INDMs showed relative greater right FA during their mothers’ and strangers’ sad <span class="elsevierStyleItalic">vs.</span> happy faces \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Bornstein et al., 2011<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0290"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">14</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">28 (14 IDM) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">5.1 mo \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Habituation and visual paired comparison procedure \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">IDMs and INDMs habituated to neutral and happy faces<br>IDMs failed to discriminate between neutral and happy faces following habituation \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Striano et al., 2002<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0425"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">41</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">46 (Maternal depression analyzed as a continuous variable) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">6 mo \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Visual paired comparison procedure comparing neutral from progressively higher intensities of smiling and frowning faces.<br>Mother-infant interaction and infant behavior coding \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Infants of mothers’ with higher depression scores showed greater looking preference for all smiling faces, and to high intensity smiling and frowning expressions<br>Infants of mothers’ with higher depression scores looked longer at their own mothers while they were smiling \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr><tr title="table-row"><td class="td-with-role" title="table-entry ; entry_with_role_rowhead " align="left" valign="top">Otte et al., 2015<a class="elsevierStyleCrossRef" href="#bib0280"><span class="elsevierStyleSup">12</span></a> \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">81 (Maternal anxiety analyzed as a continuous variable) \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">10.1 mo \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Multimodal face–voice compounds<br>ERP recorded with EEG during face–voice compounds \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td><td class="td" title="table-entry " align="left" valign="top">Infants of mothers’ with higher maternal anxiety scores showed larger P350 and P150 amplitudes after fearful vocalizations, preceded by either happy or fearful faces. \t\t\t\t\t\t\n \t\t\t\t</td></tr></tbody></table> """ ] "imagenFichero" => array:1 [ 0 => "xTab1351865.png" ] ] ] "notaPie" => array:1 [ 0 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "tblfn0005" "etiqueta" => "a" "nota" => "<p class="elsevierStyleNotepara" id="npar0005">Mean age is presented in hours or months.</p>" ] ] ] "descripcion" => array:1 [ "en" => "<p id="spar0045" class="elsevierStyleSimplePara elsevierViewall">Studies of behavior and neural correlates on infants of depressed and/or anxious mothers.</p>" ] ] ] "bibliografia" => array:2 [ "titulo" => "References" "seccion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "identificador" => "bibs0005" "bibliografiaReferencia" => array:44 [ 0 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0225" "etiqueta" => "1" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Tuning the developing brain to social signals of emotions" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:2 [ 0 => "J.M. Leppänen" 1 => "C.A. Nelson" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1038/nrn2554" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Nat Rev Neurosci" "fecha" => "2009" "volumen" => "10" "paginaInicial" => "37" "paginaFinal" => "47" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19050711" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 1 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0230" "etiqueta" => "2" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Behavioral and neural representation of emotional facial expressions across the lifespan" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:3 [ 0 => "L.H. Somerville" 1 => "N. Fani" 2 => "E.B. McClure-Tone" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1080/87565641.2010.549865" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Dev Neuropsychol" "fecha" => "2011" "volumen" => "36" "paginaInicial" => "408" "paginaFinal" => "428" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21516541" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 2 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0235" "etiqueta" => "3" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "What's in a look?" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => "R.N. Aslin" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00563.x" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Dev Sci" "fecha" => "2007" "volumen" => "10" "paginaInicial" => "48" "paginaFinal" => "53" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17181699" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 3 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0240" "etiqueta" => "4" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "The perception of smiling and its experiential correlates in three-month-old infants" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:3 [ 0 => "A. Kuchuk" 1 => "M. Vibbert" 2 => "M.H. Bornstein" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Child Dev" "fecha" => "1986" "volumen" => "57" "paginaInicial" => "1054" "paginaFinal" => "1061" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3757600" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 4 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0245" "etiqueta" => "5" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "The development of emotion perception in face and voice during infancy" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => "T. Grossmann" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.3233/RNN-2010-0499" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Restor Neurol Neurosci" "fecha" => "2010" "volumen" => "28" "paginaInicial" => "219" "paginaFinal" => "236" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20404410" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 5 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0250" "etiqueta" => "6" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "The perception of facial expressions by the three-month-old" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:2 [ 0 => "M.E. Barrera" 1 => "D. Maurer" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Child Dev" "fecha" => "1981" "volumen" => "52" "paginaInicial" => "203" "paginaFinal" => "206" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7238144" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 6 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0255" "etiqueta" => "7" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Anxiety, depression and stress in pregnancy: implications for mothers, children, research, and practice" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:2 [ 0 => "C. Dunkel Schetter" 1 => "L. Tanner" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283503680" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Curr Opin Psychiatry" "fecha" => "2012" "volumen" => "25" "paginaInicial" => "141" "paginaFinal" => "148" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22262028" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 7 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0260" "etiqueta" => "8" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Maternal depression and parenting behavior: a meta-analytic review" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:4 [ 0 => "M.C. Lovejoy" 1 => "P.A. Graczyk" 2 => "E. O’Hare" 3 => "G. Neuman" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:7 [ "tituloSerie" => "Clin Psychol Rev" "fecha" => "2000" "volumen" => "20" "paginaInicial" => "561" "paginaFinal" => "592" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10860167" "web" => "Medline" ] ] "itemHostRev" => array:3 [ "pii" => "S0161642015008441" "estado" => "S300" "issn" => "01616420" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 8 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0265" "etiqueta" => "9" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "A systematic review of the effects of postnatal maternal anxiety on children" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:3 [ 0 => "C. Glasheen" 1 => "G.A. Richardson" 2 => "A. Fabio" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1007/s00737-009-0109-y" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Arch Womens Ment Health" "fecha" => "2010" "volumen" => "13" "paginaInicial" => "61" "paginaFinal" => "74" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19789953" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 9 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0270" "etiqueta" => "10" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Prenatal depression effects on early development: a review" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => "T. Field" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.09.008" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infant Behav Dev" "fecha" => "2011" "volumen" => "34" "paginaInicial" => "1" "paginaFinal" => "14" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20970195" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 10 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0275" "etiqueta" => "11" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Pregnancy anxiety and comorbid depression and anger: effects on the fetus and neonate" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => true "autores" => array:6 [ 0 => "T. Field" 1 => "M. Diego" 2 => "M. Hernandez-Reif" 3 => "S. Schanberg" 4 => "C. Kuhn" 5 => "R. Yando" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1002/da.10071" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Depress Anxiety" "fecha" => "2003" "volumen" => "17" "paginaInicial" => "140" "paginaFinal" => "151" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12768648" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 11 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0280" "etiqueta" => "12" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Multimodal processing of emotional information in 9-month-old infants II: prenatal exposure to maternal anxiety" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:4 [ 0 => "R.A. Otte" 1 => "F.C. Donkers" 2 => "M.A. Braeken" 3 => "B.R. Van den Bergh" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1016/j.bandc.2014.12.001" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Brain Cogn" "fecha" => "2015" "volumen" => "95" "paginaInicial" => "107" "paginaFinal" => "117" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25839110" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 12 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0285" "etiqueta" => "13" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Depressed mothers’ touching increases infants’ positive affect and attention in still–face interactions" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:4 [ 0 => "M. Peláez-Nogueras" 1 => "T.M. Field" 2 => "Z. Hossain" 3 => "J. Pickens" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Child Dev" "fecha" => "1996" "volumen" => "67" "paginaInicial" => "1780" "paginaFinal" => "1792" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8890507" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 13 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0290" "etiqueta" => "14" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Discrimination of facial expression by 5-month-old infants of nondepressed and clinically depressed mothers" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:4 [ 0 => "M.H. Bornstein" 1 => "M.E. Arterberry" 2 => "C. Mash" 3 => "N. Manian" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.10.002" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infant Behav Dev" "fecha" => "2011" "volumen" => "34" "paginaInicial" => "100" "paginaFinal" => "106" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21112092" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 14 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0295" "etiqueta" => "15" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Depressed mothers’ infants are less responsive to faces and voices" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:3 [ 0 => "T. Field" 1 => "M. Diego" 2 => "M. Hernandez-Reif" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.03.005" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infant Behav Dev" "fecha" => "2009" "volumen" => "32" "paginaInicial" => "239" "paginaFinal" => "244" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19439359" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 15 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0300" "etiqueta" => "16" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Relative right frontal EEG activation in 3-to 6-month-old infants of depressed mothers. Special Section: Parental depression and distress: implications for development in infancy, childhood, and adolescence" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:4 [ 0 => "T. Field" 1 => "N. Fox" 2 => "J. Pickens" 3 => "T. Nawrocki" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:5 [ "tituloSerie" => "Dev Psychol" "fecha" => "1995" "volumen" => "31" "paginaInicial" => "358" "paginaFinal" => "363" ] ] ] ] ] ] 16 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0305" "etiqueta" => "17" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Crossmodal integration of emotional information from face and voice in the infant brain" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:3 [ 0 => "T. Grossmann" 1 => "T. Striano" 2 => "A.D. Friederici" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00494.x" "Revista" => array:7 [ "tituloSerie" => "Dev Sci" "fecha" => "2006" "volumen" => "9" "paginaInicial" => "309" "paginaFinal" => "315" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16669802" "web" => "Medline" ] ] "itemHostRev" => array:3 [ "pii" => "S1695403314005487" "estado" => "S300" "issn" => "16954033" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 17 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0310" "etiqueta" => "18" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => "T.M. Field" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:5 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infant Behav Dev" "fecha" => "1984" "volumen" => "7" "paginaInicial" => "527" "paginaFinal" => "532" ] ] ] ] ] ] 18 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0315" "etiqueta" => "19" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Still–face and separation effects on depressed mother–infant interactions" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => true "autores" => array:6 [ 0 => "T. Field" 1 => "M. Hernandez-Reif" 2 => "M. Diego" 3 => "L. Feijo" 4 => "Y. Vera" 5 => "K. Gil" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:5 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infant Ment Health J" "fecha" => "2007" "volumen" => "28" "paginaInicial" => "314" "paginaFinal" => "323" ] ] ] ] ] ] 19 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0320" "etiqueta" => "20" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => "R.J. Davidson" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:7 [ "tituloSerie" => "Brain Cogn" "fecha" => "1992" "volumen" => "20" "paginaInicial" => "125" "paginaFinal" => "151" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1389117" "web" => "Medline" ] ] "itemHostRev" => array:3 [ "pii" => "S0300289609002531" "estado" => "S300" "issn" => "03002896" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 20 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0325" "etiqueta" => "21" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Affective neuroscience and psychophysiology: toward a synthesis" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => "R.J. Davidson" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Psychophysiology" "fecha" => "2003" "volumen" => "40" "paginaInicial" => "655" "paginaFinal" => "665" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14696720" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 21 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0330" "etiqueta" => "22" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "EEG in 1-week, 1-month and 3-month-old infants of depressed and non-depressed mothers" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:3 [ 0 => "M.A. Diego" 1 => "N.A. Jones" 2 => "T. Field" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.09.007" "Revista" => array:7 [ "tituloSerie" => "Biol Psychol" "fecha" => "2010" "volumen" => "83" "paginaInicial" => "7" "paginaFinal" => "14" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19782119" "web" => "Medline" ] ] "itemHostRev" => array:3 [ "pii" => "S0161642016002475" "estado" => "S300" "issn" => "01616420" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 22 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0335" "etiqueta" => "23" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Patterns of brain electrical activity during facial signs of emotion in 10-month-old infants" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:2 [ 0 => "N.A. Fox" 1 => "R.J. Davidson" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Dev Psychol" "fecha" => "1988" "volumen" => "24" "paginaInicial" => "230" "paginaFinal" => "236" "itemHostRev" => array:3 [ "pii" => "S1470204513703164" "estado" => "S300" "issn" => "14702045" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 23 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0340" "etiqueta" => "24" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "EEG responses to mock facial expressions by infants of depressed mothers" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => true "autores" => array:6 [ 0 => "M.A. Diego" 1 => "T. Field" 2 => "N.A. Jones" 3 => "M. Hernandez-Reif" 4 => "C. Cullen" 5 => "S. Schanberg" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:5 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infant Behav Dev" "fecha" => "2004" "volumen" => "27" "paginaInicial" => "150" "paginaFinal" => "162" ] ] ] ] ] ] 24 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0345" "etiqueta" => "25" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Use of event-related potentials in the study of typical and atypical development" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:2 [ 0 => "C.A. Nelson 3rd." 1 => "J.P. McCleery" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1097/CHI.0b013e318185a6d8" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry" "fecha" => "2008" "volumen" => "47" "paginaInicial" => "1252" "paginaFinal" => "1261" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18827722" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 25 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0350" "etiqueta" => "26" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "The development and neural bases of face recognition" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => "C.A. Nelson" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:5 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infant Child Dev" "fecha" => "2001" "volumen" => "10" "paginaInicial" => "3" "paginaFinal" => "18" ] ] ] ] ] ] 26 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0355" "etiqueta" => "27" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Neural correlates of infants’ visual responsiveness to facial expressions of emotion" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:2 [ 0 => "C.A. Nelson" 1 => "M. De Haan" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199611)29:7<577::AID-DEV3>3.0.CO;2-R" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Dev Psychobiol" "fecha" => "1996" "volumen" => "29" "paginaInicial" => "577" "paginaFinal" => "595" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8911773" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 27 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0360" "etiqueta" => "28" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Maturation of the auditory event-related potentials during the first year of life" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:6 [ 0 => "E. Kushnerenko" 1 => "R. Ceponiene" 2 => "P. Balan" 3 => "V. Fellman" 4 => "M. Huotilaine" 5 => "R. Näätäne" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Neuroreport" "fecha" => "2002" "volumen" => "13" "paginaInicial" => "47" "paginaFinal" => "51" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11924892" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 28 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0365" "etiqueta" => "29" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Recognition, discrimination and categorization of smiling by 5-month-old infants" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:2 [ 0 => "M.H. Bornstein" 1 => "M.E. Arterberry" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:5 [ "tituloSerie" => "Dev Sci" "fecha" => "2003" "volumen" => "5" "paginaInicial" => "585" "paginaFinal" => "599" ] ] ] ] ] ] 29 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0370" "etiqueta" => "30" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Discrimination and imitation of facial expression by neonates" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:4 [ 0 => "T.M. Field" 1 => "R. Woodson" 2 => "R. Greenberg" 3 => "D. Cohen" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Science" "fecha" => "1982" "volumen" => "218" "paginaInicial" => "179" "paginaFinal" => "181" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7123230" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 30 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0375" "etiqueta" => "31" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by term and preterm neonates" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:6 [ 0 => "T.M. Field" 1 => "R. Woodson" 2 => "D. Cohen" 3 => "R. Greenberg" 4 => "R. Garcia" 5 => "K. Collins" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:5 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infant Behav Dev" "fecha" => "1983" "volumen" => "6" "paginaInicial" => "485" "paginaFinal" => "489" ] ] ] ] ] ] 31 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0380" "etiqueta" => "32" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "The perception of facial expressions in newborns" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:4 [ 0 => "T. Farroni" 1 => "E. Menon" 2 => "S. Rigato" 3 => "M.H. Johnson" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1080/17405620601046832" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Eur J Dev Psychol" "fecha" => "2007" "volumen" => "4" "paginaInicial" => "2" "paginaFinal" => "13" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20228970" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 32 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0385" "etiqueta" => "33" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Emergence of enhanced attention to fearful faces between 5 and 7 months of age" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:4 [ 0 => "M.J. Peltola" 1 => "J.M. Leppänen" 2 => "S. Mäki" 3 => "J.K. Hietanen" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1093/scan/nsn046" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci" "fecha" => "2009" "volumen" => "4" "paginaInicial" => "134" "paginaFinal" => "142" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19174536" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 33 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0390" "etiqueta" => "34" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Infants’ discrimination of vocal expressions: contributions of auditory and visual information" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:2 [ 0 => "A.S. Walker-Andrews" 1 => "E. Lennon" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:5 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infant Behav Dev" "fecha" => "1991" "volumen" => "14" "paginaInicial" => "131" "paginaFinal" => "142" ] ] ] ] ] ] 34 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0395" "etiqueta" => "35" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Taste-elicited changes in facial signs of emotion and the asymmetry of brain electrical activity in human newborns" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:2 [ 0 => "N.A. Fox" 1 => "R.J. Davidson" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Neuropsychologia" "fecha" => "1986" "volumen" => "24" "paginaInicial" => "417" "paginaFinal" => "422" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3736825" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 35 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0400" "etiqueta" => "36" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Asymmetrical brain activity discriminates between positive and negative affective stimuli in human infants" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:2 [ 0 => "R.J. Davidson" 1 => "N.A. Fox" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Science" "fecha" => "1982" "volumen" => "218" "paginaInicial" => "1235" "paginaFinal" => "1237" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7146906" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 36 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0405" "etiqueta" => "37" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Right frontal brain activity, cortisol, and withdrawal behavior in 6-month-old infants" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:6 [ 0 => "K.A. Buss" 1 => "J.R. Schumacher" 2 => "I. Dolski" 3 => "N.H. Kalin" 4 => "H.H. Goldsmith" 5 => "R.J. Davidson" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Behav Neurosci" "fecha" => "2003" "volumen" => "117" "paginaInicial" => "11" "paginaFinal" => "20" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12619903" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 37 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0410" "etiqueta" => "38" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Multimodal processing of emotional information in 9-month-old infants I: emotional faces and voices" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:4 [ 0 => "R.A. Otte" 1 => "F.C. Donkers" 2 => "M.A. Braeken" 3 => "B.R. Van den Bergh" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1016/j.bandc.2014.09.007" "Revista" => array:7 [ "tituloSerie" => "Brain Cogn" "fecha" => "2015" "volumen" => "95" "paginaInicial" => "99" "paginaFinal" => "106" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25839109" "web" => "Medline" ] ] "itemHostRev" => array:3 [ "pii" => "S1470204514704097" "estado" => "S300" "issn" => "14702045" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 38 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0415" "etiqueta" => "39" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Depressed mothers’ newborns show longer habituation and fail to show face/voice preference" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:4 [ 0 => "M. Hernandez-Reif" 1 => "T. Field" 2 => "M. Diego" 3 => "S. Largie" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:5 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infant Ment Health J" "fecha" => "2002" "volumen" => "23" "paginaInicial" => "643" "paginaFinal" => "653" ] ] ] ] ] ] 39 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0420" "etiqueta" => "40" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Happy faces are habituated more slowly by infants of depressed mothers" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:5 [ 0 => "M. Hernandez-Reif" 1 => "T. Field" 2 => "M. Diego" 3 => "Y. Vera" 4 => "J. Pickens" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "doi" => "10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.07.003" "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infant Behav Dev" "fecha" => "2006" "volumen" => "29" "paginaInicial" => "131" "paginaFinal" => "135" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17138268" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 40 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0425" "etiqueta" => "41" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Maternal depressive symptoms and 6-month-old infants’ sensitivity to facial expressions" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:3 [ 0 => "T. Striano" 1 => "P.A. Brennan" 2 => "E.J. Vanman" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Infancy" "fecha" => "2002" "volumen" => "3" "paginaInicial" => "115" "paginaFinal" => "126" "itemHostRev" => array:3 [ "pii" => "S1470204513705485" "estado" => "S300" "issn" => "14702045" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] 41 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0430" "etiqueta" => "42" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Facial expressions and vagal tone of infants of depressed and non-depressed mothers" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:2 [ 0 => "J.N. Pickens" 1 => "T. Field" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:5 [ "tituloSerie" => "Early Dev Parent" "fecha" => "1995" "volumen" => "4" "paginaInicial" => "83" "paginaFinal" => "89" ] ] ] ] ] ] 42 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0435" "etiqueta" => "43" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Facial expression and EEG responses to happy and sad faces/voices by 3-month-old infants of depressed mothers" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => false "autores" => array:5 [ 0 => "T. Field" 1 => "J. Pickens" 2 => "N. Fox" 3 => "J. Gonzalez" 4 => "T. Nawrocki" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:5 [ "tituloSerie" => "Br J Dev Psychol" "fecha" => "1998" "volumen" => "16" "paginaInicial" => "485" "paginaFinal" => "494" ] ] ] ] ] ] 43 => array:3 [ "identificador" => "bib0440" "etiqueta" => "44" "referencia" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "contribucion" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Facial expressions and EEG in infants of intrusive and withdrawn mothers with depressive symptoms" "autores" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "etal" => true "autores" => array:6 [ 0 => "M.A. Diego" 1 => "T. Field" 2 => "S. Hart" 3 => "M. Hernandez-Reif" 4 => "N. Jones" 5 => "C. Cullen" ] ] ] ] ] "host" => array:1 [ 0 => array:1 [ "Revista" => array:6 [ "tituloSerie" => "Depress Anxiety" "fecha" => "2002" "volumen" => "15" "paginaInicial" => "10" "paginaFinal" => "17" "link" => array:1 [ 0 => array:2 [ "url" => "https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11816047" "web" => "Medline" ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] ] "idiomaDefecto" => "en" "url" => "/00217557/00000092000003S1/v2_201702240034/S0021755716000528/v2_201702240034/en/main.assets" "Apartado" => array:4 [ "identificador" => "10176" "tipo" => "SECCION" "en" => array:2 [ "titulo" => "Review articles" "idiomaDefecto" => true ] "idiomaDefecto" => "en" ] "PDF" => "https://static.elsevier.es/multimedia/00217557/00000092000003S1/v2_201702240034/S0021755716000528/v2_201702240034/en/main.pdf?idApp=UINPBA000049&text.app=https://jped.elsevier.es/" "EPUB" => "https://multimedia.elsevier.es/PublicationsMultimediaV1/item/epub/S0021755716000528?idApp=UINPBA000049" ]
Year/Month | Html | Total | |
---|---|---|---|
2024 November | 6 | 5 | 11 |
2024 October | 16 | 46 | 62 |
2024 September | 37 | 29 | 66 |
2024 August | 67 | 40 | 107 |
2024 July | 36 | 46 | 82 |
2024 June | 25 | 27 | 52 |
2024 May | 20 | 20 | 40 |
2024 April | 24 | 23 | 47 |
2024 March | 15 | 19 | 34 |
2024 February | 19 | 25 | 44 |
2024 January | 14 | 27 | 41 |
2023 December | 11 | 26 | 37 |
2023 November | 28 | 36 | 64 |
2023 October | 25 | 31 | 56 |
2023 September | 27 | 47 | 74 |
2023 August | 16 | 16 | 32 |
2023 July | 12 | 15 | 27 |
2023 June | 18 | 19 | 37 |
2023 May | 24 | 14 | 38 |
2023 April | 15 | 11 | 26 |
2023 March | 41 | 18 | 59 |
2023 February | 25 | 11 | 36 |
2023 January | 15 | 22 | 37 |
2022 December | 40 | 21 | 61 |
2022 November | 20 | 24 | 44 |
2022 October | 33 | 31 | 64 |
2022 September | 21 | 30 | 51 |
2022 August | 21 | 19 | 40 |
2022 July | 21 | 24 | 45 |
2022 June | 16 | 25 | 41 |
2022 May | 39 | 26 | 65 |
2022 April | 24 | 45 | 69 |
2022 March | 23 | 34 | 57 |
2022 February | 13 | 27 | 40 |
2022 January | 10 | 18 | 28 |
2021 December | 12 | 16 | 28 |
2021 November | 9 | 12 | 21 |
2021 October | 9 | 15 | 24 |
2021 September | 6 | 12 | 18 |
2021 August | 6 | 6 | 12 |
2021 July | 4 | 6 | 10 |
2021 June | 3 | 3 | 6 |
2021 May | 9 | 13 | 22 |
2021 April | 5 | 5 | 10 |
2021 March | 11 | 13 | 24 |
2021 February | 3 | 5 | 8 |
2021 January | 4 | 6 | 10 |
2020 December | 5 | 7 | 12 |
2020 November | 8 | 13 | 21 |
2020 October | 5 | 6 | 11 |
2020 September | 10 | 9 | 19 |
2020 August | 4 | 3 | 7 |
2020 July | 4 | 4 | 8 |
2020 June | 5 | 4 | 9 |
2020 May | 10 | 2 | 12 |
2020 April | 86 | 11 | 97 |
2020 March | 4 | 4 | 8 |
2020 February | 7 | 6 | 13 |
2020 January | 10 | 12 | 22 |
2019 December | 9 | 8 | 17 |
2019 November | 8 | 6 | 14 |
2019 October | 6 | 10 | 16 |
2019 September | 8 | 7 | 15 |
2019 August | 5 | 4 | 9 |
2019 July | 6 | 6 | 12 |
2019 June | 11 | 10 | 21 |
2019 May | 6 | 8 | 14 |
2019 April | 9 | 13 | 22 |
2019 March | 8 | 6 | 14 |
2019 February | 5 | 5 | 10 |
2019 January | 4 | 8 | 12 |
2018 December | 6 | 17 | 23 |
2018 November | 24 | 4 | 28 |
2018 October | 130 | 16 | 146 |
2018 September | 2 | 17 | 19 |
2018 August | 6 | 9 | 15 |
2018 July | 4 | 7 | 11 |
2018 June | 6 | 5 | 11 |
2018 May | 13 | 15 | 28 |
2018 April | 5 | 4 | 9 |
2018 March | 7 | 1 | 8 |
2018 February | 4 | 3 | 7 |
2018 January | 7 | 2 | 9 |
2017 December | 1 | 0 | 1 |
2017 November | 9 | 1 | 10 |
2017 October | 7 | 2 | 9 |
2017 September | 2 | 2 | 4 |
2017 August | 3 | 1 | 4 |
2017 July | 1 | 2 | 3 |
2017 June | 7 | 7 | 14 |
2017 May | 3 | 3 | 6 |
2017 April | 3 | 37 | 40 |
2017 March | 2 | 12 | 14 |
2017 February | 1 | 0 | 1 |
2017 January | 4 | 2 | 6 |
2016 December | 2 | 9 | 11 |
2016 November | 0 | 8 | 8 |
2016 October | 2 | 11 | 13 |
2016 September | 2 | 10 | 12 |
2016 August | 4 | 2 | 6 |
2016 July | 7 | 3 | 10 |
2016 June | 3 | 8 | 11 |
2016 May | 10 | 11 | 21 |
2016 April | 16 | 12 | 28 |